Skip to main content
Grundlagen

Wie Babys vom Gesichtsausdruck der Mutter lernen

Eine experimentelle Untersuchung zur familialen Transmission von Ängsten

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1026/0942-5403/a000002

Ätiologiemodelle zur Entstehung von Angststörungen messen neben genetischen Faktoren Umwelterfahrungen eine besondere Bedeutung bei. Anhand eines Messwiederholungsdesigns wurde geprüft, ob die mütterliche Stimmung, die durch Videoausschnitte mit beängstigenden oder neutralem Charakter experimentell manipuliert wurde, das Verhalten des Kindes (8 – 13 Monate) in einer für das Kind ambivalenten Situation, der visuellen Klippe, beeinflusst. Es wurden 25 Mutter-Kind-Paare in die Studie einbezogen. Eine Regressionsanalyse zeigte, dass das Temperament des Kindes und habituelle Depression der Mutter signifikante Prädiktoren für das Verhalten des Kleinkindes in der visuellen Klippe waren. Darüber hinaus zeigte sich tendenziell die akute negative Befindlichkeit der Mutter als Prädiktor. Die Ergebnisse weisen auf die Bedeutung mütterlicher Befindlichkeit und Temperamentsmerkmalen für das Verhalten von Kleinkindern in ambivalenten und unvertrauten Situationen hin.


Learning from mother’s face: An experimental examination of the transgenerational transmission of anxiety

In addition to genetic factors, etiological models of anxiety disorders underscore the importance of observational learning in the development of anxiety. The present study examines the transgenerational transmission of anxiety disorders using an experimental repeated measures design. We used the visual cliff paradigm to study the relationship between maternal emotions and infant’s social referencing behavior in an ambiguous situation. Our data, collected from 25 mothers with their 8-13-month-old infants, indicated that infants’ temperament and mothers’ depressive mood explained a significant amount of variance in infants’ behavior. Further, increases in “strain” and “anxiety” by trend had additional effects on infants’ behavior. Findings indicate the relevance of mothers’ mental state and infants’ temperament for infant behavior in ambivalent and unfamiliar situations.

Literatur

  • Askew, C. , Field, A. P. (2007). Vicarious learning and the development of fears in childhood. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 2616 – 2627. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bates, J. E. , Freeland, C. A. , Lounsbury, M. L. (1979). Measurement of infant difficultness. Child Development, 50, 794 – 803. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bögels & Brechman-Toussaint (2006). Family issues in child anxiety: Attachment, family functioning, parental rearing and beliefs. Clinical Psychology Review, 7, 834 – 856. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • de Rosnay, M. , Cooper, P. J. , Tsigaras, N. & Murray, L. (2006). Transmission of social anxiety from mother to infant: An experimental study using a social referencing paradigm. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1165 – 1175. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Field, A. P. , Lawson, J. (2003). Fear information and the development of fears during childhood: Effects on implicit fear responses and behavioural avoidance. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 1277 – 1293. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Gibson, E. J. , Walk, R. D. (1960). The ”visual cliff”. Scientific American, 202, 64 – 71. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Henry, J. D. , Crawford, J. R. (2005). The short-form version of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS-21): Construct validity and normative data in a large non-clinical sample. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44, 227 – 239. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Köppe, E. (2001). Glückliche Eltern, liebe Kinder: Auswirkungen von Partnerschaft und psychischer Symptomatik der Eltern auf das Verhalten ihrer Kinder. Dissertation, Technische Universität Braunschweig. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Lovibond, S. H. , Lovibond, P. F. (1995). Manual for the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales. Sydney: Psychology Foundation (unpublished manuscript). First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Moses, L. J. , Baldwin, D. A. , Rosicky, J. G. , Tidball, G. (2001). Evidence for referential understanding in the emotions domain at twelve and eighteen months. Child Development, 72, 718 – 735. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Murray, L. , de Rosnay, M. , Pearson, J. , Bergeron, C. , Schofield, E. , Royal-Lawson, M. , Cooper, P. J. (2008). Intergenerational Transmission of social anxiety: The role of social referencing processes in infancy. Child Delvelopment, 79, 1049 – 1064. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Olino, T. M. , Pettit, J. W. , Klein, D. N. , Allen, N. B. , Seeley, J. R. , Lewinsohn, P. M. (2008). Influence of parental and grandparental major depressive disorder on behavior problems in early childhood: a three-generation study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 47, 53 – 60. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Petermann, U. , Petermann, F. (2009). Soziale Angst/Soziale Unsicherheit. Kindheit und Entwicklung, 18, 1 – 5. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Rachman, S. (1977). The conditioning theory of fear-acquisition: a critical examination. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 15, 375 – 387. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Schneider, S. (2004). Risikofaktoren für die Entwicklung von Angststörungen. In S. Schneider (Hrsg.), Angststörungen bei Kindern und Jugendlichen: Grundlagen und Behandlung, Berlin: Springer. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Schneider, S. , Unnewehr, S. & Margraf, J. (2007). Kinder-DIPS für DSM-IV-TR. Diagnostisches Interview bei psychischen Störungen im Kindes- und Jugendalter. Basel: Manuskript. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Sorce, J. F. , Emde, R. N. , Campos, J. J. , Klinnert, M. D. (1985). Maternal emotional signaling: Its effect on the visual cliff behavior of 1-year-olds. Developmental Psychology, 21, 195 – 200. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Sroufe, L. A. (1996). Emotional development: The organization of emotional life in the early years. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Stenberg, G. (2003). Effects of maternal inattentiveness on infant social referencing. Infant and Child Development, 12, 399 – 419. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Stenberg, G. , Hagekull, B. (1997). Social referencing and mood modification in 1-year-olds. Infant Behavior and Development, 20, 209 – 217. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Vaish, A. , Striano, T. (2004). Is visual reference necessary? Contributions of facial versus vocal cues in 12-month-olds’ social referencing behaviour. Developmental Science, 7, 261 – 269. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Wessel, M.A. , Cobb, J.C. , Jackson, E.B. , Harris, G.S. Jr. , Detwiler, A.C. (1954). Paroxysmal fussing in infancy, sometimes called colic. Pediatrics, 14, 421 – 435. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar