Skip to main content
Freier Beitrag

Kognitive Fähigkeiten und Lesekompetenz

Zur Bedeutung der Intelligenz für die Güte der Lesefertigkeit

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1026/0942-5403/a000215

Zusammenfassung. In der vorliegenden Studie wird der Zusammenhang zwischen kognitiven Fähigkeiten und der Lesefertigkeit bei Schulkindern untersucht. Anhand einer Stichprobe von 101 deutschsprachigen Kindern im Alter zwischen 7 und 16 Jahren wurden Zusammenhänge zwischen den verschiedenen Indizes der Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-4th Edition, WISC-IV (Petermann & Petermann, 2011), und den Leistungen in zwei standardisierten Lesetests analysiert. Die Ergebnisse deuten auf einen umschriebenen Einfluss bestimmter Intelligenzbereiche auf die Qualität der Lesefertigkeit hin. Dies unterstützt die in den Leitlinien der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie und -psychotherapie et al. (DGKJP; 2015) empfohlene Verwendung eines IQ-Diskrepanzkriteriums, auch unter der Verwendung differenzierter Intelligenztests, wie der aktuellen Version des Wechsler Intelligenztests für Kinder und Jugendliche, WISC-IV.


Cognitive Abilities and Literacy: The Role of Intelligence in Reading Skills

Abstract. The present study aims to explore the relationship between cognitive abilities and reading skills in children and adolescents. Significant correlations have been found between the cognitive domains working memory, verbal comprehension, processing speed, and perceptual reasoning and specific reading skills, such as reading speed, fluency, and reading comprehension. Current research emphasizes the impact of biological explanations of reading difficulties as well as phonological processing. Both are closely related to cognitive performance. A low IQ score is mandatory for the diagnosis of developmental dyslexia in a clinical context. The present study analyzes the impact of IQ performance on reading and whether the current recommendations of diagnostic criteria for reading difficulties in Germany are applicable. A sample of 101 German children ranging in age from 7 to 16 years were compared regarding their IQ scores and their reading skills. Cognitive performance was assessed with the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children – 4th Edition (WISC-IV; Petermann & Petermann, 2011). Reading skills were measured using two standardized reading tests (Zürcher Lesetest – II [Petermann & Daseking, 2012], Ein Leseverständnistest für Erst- bis Sechstklässler [Lenhard & Schneider, 2006]). Correlations and multiple hierarchical regression analyses were carried out in order to quantify the impact of cognitive and other variables (gender, speech development) on reading skills. Cognitive performance and reading are correlated. Working memory has the highest impact on reading skills (r = .51–.60) followed by verbal comprehension (r = .40–.50). Our findings correspond to those of other studies focusing on similar aspects of reading. The percentage of variance explained by the regression analyses is moderate (R2 = .50). Working memory and its different aspects are closely related to processes underlying reading skills. If the acquisition of symbol coding is disturbed because of poor memory, fluent reading will not be possible. Working memory aspects include the visuospatial sketchbook, which is also part of processing speed. Along with visual perception and verbal comprehension, these cognitive skills form the basic prerequisites for learning to read. The importance of cognitive abilities for academic achievement has been acknowledged in the diagnostic criteria for dyslexia: According to ICD-10, developmental dyslexia is marked by reading achievement that falls substantially below that expected given the individual’s chronological age, measured intelligence, and age-appropriate education. The determination of the discrepancy, however, is not a fixed factor: the latest guidelines of the German Society of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics, and Psychotherapy (DGKJP) recommend the use of the IQ discrepancy as one option among others, with the German Society of Psychology (DGPs) even suggesting the rejection of the IQ discrepancy. Considering our results, we support the application of the regression model, which has been established to allow for dyslexia diagnoses even for children with high IQ scores. It integrates the correlation of cognitive performance and reading skills and provides specific cut-off scores for reading test results.

Literatur

  • Backhaus, K., Erichson, B., Plinke, W. & Weiber, R. (2011). Multivariate Analysemethoden: Eine anwendungsorientierte Einführung (13., überarb. Aufl.). Berlin: Springer. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baddeley, A. D. & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In Bower, G.Ed., The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (pp. 47 – 89). New York: Academic Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bortz, J. & Schuster, C. (2010). Statistik für Human- und Sozialwissenschaftler (7. Aufl.). Berlin: Springer. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brandenburg, J., Klesczewski, J., Fischbach, A., Büttner, G., Grube, D. & Mähler, C., et al. (2013). Arbeitsgedächtnisfunktionen von Kindern mit Minderleistungen in der Schriftsprache. Lernen und Lernstörungen, 2, 147 – 159. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Cattell, R. B. (1963). Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence – a critical experiment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 54, 1 – 22. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Coltheart, M. (2007). Modeling reading: The dual-route approach. In Snowling, M. J.Hulme, C.Eds., The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 6 – 23). Malden, MA: Blackwell. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Daneman, M. & Merikle, P. M. (1996). Working memory and language comprehension: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3, 422 – 433. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Deutsche Gesellschaft für Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie, Psychosomatik und Psychotherapie e. V. (Hrsg.). (2015). Diagnostik und Behandlung von Kindern und Jugendlichen mit Lese- und / oder Rechtschreibstörung. Evidenz- und konsensbasierte Leitlinie (S3); AWMF – Registernummer 028 – 044. Zugriff am 23. 01. 2016. Verfügbar unter http://www.awmf.org/uploads/tx_szleitlinien/028 – 044l_S3_Lese-Rechtschreibst%C3 %B6rungen_Kinder_Jugendliche_2015 – 06.pdf First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Deutsche Gesellschaft für Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie et al. (Hrsg.). (2007). Leitlinien zur Diagnostik und Therapie von psychischen Störungen im Säuglings-, Kindes- und Jugendalter (3. Aufl.). Köln: Deutscher Ärzte Verlag. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Dilling, H., Mombour, W. & Schmidt, M. H. (Hrsg.). (2010). Internationale Klassifikation psychischer Störungen ICD-10 Kapitel V (F) Klinisch-diagnostische Leitlinien. Bern: Huber. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ehri, L. C. (1999). Phases of development in learning to read words. In Oakhill, J.Beard, R.Eds., Reading development and the teaching of reading: A psychological perspective (pp. 79 – 108). Malden, MA: Blackwell. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ehri, L. C. (2005). Development of sight word reading: phases and findings. In Snowling, M. J.Hulme, C.Eds., The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 135 – 154). Malden, MA: Blackwell. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Flanagan, D. P. & Kaufman, A. S. (2004). Essentials of WISC-IV Assessment. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In Patterson, K. E.Marshall, J. C.Coltheart, M.Eds., Surface dyslexia: Neuropsychological and cognitive studies of phonological reading (pp. 301 – 330). London: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Garcia, J. R. & Cain, K. (2014). Decoding and reading comprehension: A meta-analysis to identify which reader and assessment characteristics influence the strength of the relationship in English. Review of Educational Research, 84, 74 – 111. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gough, P. B. & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education,7, 6 – 10. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Grimm, H. (2012). Störungen der Sprachentwicklung: Grundlagen – Ursachen – Diagnose – Intervention – Prävention (3., überarb. Aufl.). Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hagtvet, B. E. (2003). Listening comprehension and reading comprehension in poor decoders: Evidence for the importance of syntactic and semantic skills as well as phonological skills. Reading and Writing, 16, 505 – 539. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hutton, U. M. & Towse, J. N. (2001). Short-term memory and working memory as indices of children’s cognitive skills. Memory, 9, 383 – 394. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jimenez, J. E., Siegel, L., O’Shanahan, I. & Ford, L. (2009). The relative roles of IQ and cognitive processes in reading disability. Educational Psychology, 29, 27 – 43. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jimenez, J. E., Siegel, L. S. & Lopez, M. R. (2003). The relationship between IQ and reading disabilities in English-speaking Canadian and Spanish children. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36, 15 – 23. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kendeou, P., Savage, R. & Broek, P. van den (2009). Revisiting the simple view of reading. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 353 – 370. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Klossek, J. (2010). Modellierung von (globalem) Leseverständnis. In Rupp, G.J., BoelmannFrickel, D.Hrsg., Aspekte literarischen Lernens (S. 95 – 112). Berlin: LIT. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Landerl, K., Ramus, F., Moll, K., Lyytinen, H., Leppänen, P. H. T. & Lohvansuu, K., et al. (2013). Predictors of developmental dyslexia in European orthographies with varying complexity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54, 686 – 694. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lenhard, W. & Schneider, W. (2006). Ein Lese-Verständnistest für Erst- bis Sechstklässler. Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Maehler, C. & Schuchardt, K. (2011). Working memory in children with learning disabilities: Rethinking the criterion of discrepancy. International Journal of Disability, Development and Education, 58, 5 – 17. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McArthur, G. M., Hogben, J. H., Edwards, V. T., Heath, S. M. & Mengler, E. D. (2000). On the “Specifics“ of specific reading disability and specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 869 – 874. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McVay, J. C. & Kane, M. J. (2012). Why does working memory capacity predict variation in reading comprehension? On the influence of mind wandering and executive attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 141, 302 – 320. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Moll, K., Wallner, R. & Landerl, K. (2012). Kognitive Korrelate der Lese-, Leserechtschreib- und Rechtschreibstörung. Lernen und Lernstörungen, 1, 7 – 19. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Petermann, F. & Daseking, M. (2012). Zürcher Lesetest-II (ZLT-II). Bern: Huber. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Petermann, F. & Petermann, U. (Hrsg.). (2011). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children – Fourth Edition (WISC-IV). Frankfurt: Pearson Assessment. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ramus, F., Marshall, C. R., Rosen, S. & Lely, H. K. J. van der (2013). Phonological deficits in specific language impairment and developmental dyslexia: Towards a multidimensional model. Brain, 136, 630 – 645. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ruland, A., Willmes, K. & Günther, T. (2012). Zusammenhang zwischen Aufmerksamkeitsdefiziten und Lese-Rechtschreibschwäche. Kindheit und Entwicklung, 21, 57 – 63. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Scerri, T. S. & Schulte-Körne, G. (2010). Genetics of developmental dyslexia. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 19, 179 – 197. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schneider, W. (Hrsg.). (2008). Entwicklung von der Kindheit bis zum Erwachsenenalter: Befunde der Münchner Längsschnittstudie LOGIK. Weinheim: Beltz. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Schulte-Körne, G., Deimel, W. & Remschmidt, H. (2001). Zur Diagnostik der Lese-Rechtschreibstörung. Zeitschrift für Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie und Psychotherapie, 29, 113 – 116. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Seymour, P. H. K., Aro, M., Erskine, J. M., Wimmer, H., Leybaert, J. & Elbro, C., et al. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 143 – 174. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Snowling, M. J. (2001). Language and literacy skills: Who is at risk and why? In Bishop, D. V. M.Leonard, L. B.Eds., Speech and language impairment in children: Causes, characteristics, intervention and outcome (pp. 245 – 259). Hove, East Sussex: Psychology Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1991). Conceptual and empirical problems with discrepancy definitions of reading-disability. Learning Disability Quarterly, 14, 269 – 280. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Suchodoletz, W. von (2013). Sprech- und Sprachstörungen. Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Tischler, T., Daseking, M. & Petermann, F. (2013). Effekt der Schulform auf die Entwicklung der Lesegeschwindigkeit. Diagnostica, 59, 215 – 226. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Wagner, R. K. & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading-skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192 – 212. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wang, S. & Gathercole, S. E. (2013). Working Memory deficits in children with reading difficulties: Memory span and dual task coordination. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115, 188 – 197. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar