Skip to main content
Original Articles and Reviews

Performance Goals and Task Performance

Integrative Considerations on the Distraction Hypothesis

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040/a000281

Abstract. Recent years have seen an increased interest in understanding how high-stakes evaluative contexts, which are pervasive in the academic arena, may influence crucial outcomes such as performance and achievement. The salience of grades, as well as the importance to distinguish oneself in the eyes of teachers to have access to valued diplomas, encourages the adoption of performance-approach goals (i.e., the desire to outperform others). Consistent with literature documenting the cognitive costs of high-pressure situations, recent findings have highlighted the detrimental consequences of performance-approach goals on availability of working memory resources, pointing at distraction as the cause of this phenomenon. We review and discuss this result in the light of the achievement goal literature. We then present both methodological and theoretical arguments to clarify and reconcile the apparent contradictions between this emerging evidence and the well-documented positive impact of performance-approach goal pursuit on achievement in the classroom. Throughout, we highlight how the study of performance-approach goal-related interference has the potential to enrich our understanding of how evaluative contexts do generate distraction.

References

  • Avery, R. E. & Smillie, L. D. (2013). The impact of achievement goal states on working memory. Motivation and Emotion, 37, 39–49. doi: 10.1007/s11031-012-9287-4 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Avery, R. E., Smillie, L. D. & de Fockert, J. W. (2013). The role of working memory in achievement goal pursuit. Acta Psychologica, 144, 361–372. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.07.012 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baron, R. S., Moore, D. & Sanders, G. S. (1978). Distraction as a source of drive in social facilitation research. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 816–824. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.36.8.816 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Barron, K. E. & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2003). Revisiting the benefits of performance-approach goals in the college classroom: Exploring the role of goals in advanced college courses. International Journal of Educational Research, 39, 357–374. doi: 10.1016/j.ijer.2004.06.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Beilock, S. L. & DeCaro, M. S. (2007). From poor performance to success under stress: Working memory, strategy selection, and mathematical problem solving under pressure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 983–998. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.6.983 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Beilock, S. L., Kulp, C. A., Holt, L. E. & Carr, T. H. (2004). More on the fragility of performance: Choking under pressure in mathematical problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 584–600. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.4.584 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brophy, J. (2005). Goal theorists should move on from performance goals. Educational Psychologist, 40, 167–176. doi: 10.1207/s15326985ep4003_3 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Butler, R. & Nisan, M. (1986). Effects of no feedback, task-related comments, and grades on intrinsic motivation and performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 210–216. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.78.3.210 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cabut, S. (2015, August 24). Les carabins, dopés aux stimulants. Le Monde. Retrieved from http://www.lemonde.fr/sciences/article/2015/08/24/les-carabins-dopes-aux-stimulants_4735345_1650684.html First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Church, M. A., Elliot, A. J. & Gable, S. L. (2001). Perceptions of classroom environment, achievement goals, and achievement outcomes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 43–45. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.93.1.43 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Covington, M. V. & Omelich, C. L. (1984). Task-oriented versus competitive learning structures: Motivational and performance consequences. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 1038–1050. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.76.6.1038 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Crouzevialle, M. & Butera, F. (2013). Performance-approach goals deplete working memory and impair cognitive performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 666–678. doi: 10.1037/a0029632 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Crouzevialle, M., Smeding, A. & Butera, F. (2015). Striving for excellence sometimes hinders high achievers: Performance-approach goals deplete arithmetical performance in students with high working memory capacity. PLoS One, 10, e0137629. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0137629 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Darnon, C., Butera, F. & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2007). Achievement goals in social interactions: Learning within a mastery vs. performance goal. Motivation and Emotion, 31, 61–70. doi: 10.1007/s11031-006-9049-2 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Darnon, C., Butera, F., Mugny, G., Quiamzade, A. & Hulleman, C. S. (2009). “Too complex for me!” Why do performance-approach and performance-avoidance goals predict exam performance? European Journal of Psychology of Education, 4, 423–434. doi: 10.1007/BF03178759 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Darnon, C., Dompnier, B., Delmas, F., Pulfrey, C. & Butera, F. (2009). Achievement goal promotion at university: Social desirability and social utility of mastery and performance goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 119–134. doi: 10.1037/a0012824 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Darnon, C., Harackiewicz, J., Butera, F., Mugny, G. & Quiamzade, A. (2007). Performance-approach and performance-avoidance goals: When uncertainty makes a difference. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 813–827. doi: 10.1177/0146167207301022 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Darnon, C., Muller, D., Schrager, S. M., Pannuzzo, N. & Butera, F. (2006). Mastery and performance goals predict epistemic and relational conflict regulation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 766–776. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.98.4.766 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • DeCaro, M. S., Rotar, K. E., Kendra, M. S. & Beilock, S. L. (2010). Diagnosing and alleviating the impact of performance pressure on mathematical problem solving. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 63, 1619–1630. doi: 10.1080/17470210903474286 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Deutsch, M. (1979). Education and distributive justice: Some reflections on grading systems. The American Psychologist, 34, 391–401. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.34.5.391 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dompnier, B., Darnon, C. & Butera, F. (2009). Faking the Desire to Learn: A clarification of the link between mastery goals and academic achievement. Psychological Science, 20, 939–943. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02384.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dompnier, B., Darnon, C. & Butera, F. (2013). When performance-approach goals predict academic achievement and when they do not: A social value approach. The British Journal of Social Psychology, 52, 587–596. doi: 10.1111/bjso.12025 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dompnier, B., Darnon, C., Delmas, F. & Butera, F. (2008). Achievement goals and social judgment: The performance-approach paradox. International Review of Social Psychology, 21, 247–271. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Dweck, C. S. (1986). Motivational processes affect learning. The American Psychologist, 41, 1040–1048. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.41.10.1040 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Elliot, A. J. (2005). A conceptual history of the achievement goal construct. In A. ElliotC. DweckEds., Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 52–72). New York, NY: Guilford Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Elliot, A. J. & Church, M. A. (1997). A hierarchical model of approach and avoidance achievement motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 218–232. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.72.1.218 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Elliot, A. J. & McGregor, H. A. (1999). Test anxiety and the hierarchical model of approach and avoidance achievement motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 628–644. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.76.4.628 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Elliot, A. J. & McGregor, H. A. (2001). A 2 x 2 achievement goal framework. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 501–519. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.3.501 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Elliot, A. J., McGregor, H. A. & Gable, S. (1999). Achievement goals, study strategies, and exam performance: A mediational analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 549–563. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.91.3.549 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Elliot, A. J. & Moller, A. C. (2003). Performance-approach goals: Good or bad forms of regulation? International Journal of Educational Research, 39, 339–356. doi: 10.1016/j.ijer.2004.06.003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Elliot, A. J. & Thrash, T. (2001). Achievement goals and the hierarchical model of achievement motivation. Educational Psychology Review, 13, 139–156. doi: 10.1023/A:1009057102306 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Emmons, R. A., King, L. A. & Sheldon, K. (1993). Goal conflict and the self-regulation of action. In D. M. WegnerJ. W. PennebakerEds., Handbook of Mental Control (pp. 528–551). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Engle, R. W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 19–23. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00160 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Förster, J., Liberman, N. & Higgins, E. T. (2005). Accessibility from active and fulfilled goals. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 220–239. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2004.06.009 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fraissard, G. (2013, September 17). Stress scolaire: L’obsession de l’excellence. Le Monde. Retrieved from http://www.lemonde.fr/culture/article/2013/09/17/stress-scolaire-l-obsession-de-l-excellence_3477712_3246.html First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Freund, A. M. & Hennecke, M. (2015). On means and ends: The role of goal focus in successful goal pursuit. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24, 149–153. doi: 10.1177/0963721414559774 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fujita, K., Trope, Y., Liberman, N. & Levin-Sagi, M. (2006). Construal levels and self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 351–367. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.90.3.351 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gimmig, D., Huguet, P., Caverni, J. -P. & Cury, F. (2006). Choking under pressure and working memory capacity: When performance pressure reduces fluid intelligence. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 1005–1010. doi: 10.3758/BF03213916 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gollwitzer, P. M. (1996). The volitional benefits of planning. In P. M. GollwitzerJ. A. BarghEds., The psychology of action (pp. 287–312). New York, NY: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Gollwitzer, P. M. & Moskowitz, G. B. (1996). Goal effects on action and cognition. In E. T. HigginsA. KruglanskiEds., Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 361–399). New York, NY: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., Tauer, J. M., Carter, S. M. & Elliot, A. J. (2000). Short-term and long-term consequences of achievement goals: Predicting interest and performance over time. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 316–330. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.92.2.316 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., Tauer, J. M. & Elliot, A. J. (2002). Predicting success in college: A longitudinal study of achievement goals and ability measures as predictors of interest and performance from Freshman year through graduation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 562–575. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.94.3.562 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Harackiewicz, J. M., Durik, A. M., Barron, K. E., Linnenbrink, E. A. & Tauer, J. M. (2008). The role of achievement goals in the development of interest: Reciprocal relations between achievement goals, interest and performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 105–122. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.100.1.105 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hulleman, C. S., Schrager, S. M., Bodmann, S. M. & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2010). A meta-analytic review of achievement goal measures: Different labels for the same constructs or different constructs with similar labels? Psychological Bulletin, 136, 422–449. doi: 10.1037/a0018947 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kane, M. J., Conway, A. R. A., Hambrick, D. Z. & Engle, R. W. (2007). Variation in working memory capacity as variation in executive attention and control. In A. R. A. ConwayC. JarroldM. J. KaneA. MiyakeJ. N. TowseEds., Variation in working memory (pp. 21–48). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Klinger, E. (1975). Consequences of commitment to and disengagement from incentives. Psychological Review, 82, 1–25. doi: 10.1037/h0076171 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Klinger, E. (2009). Daydreaming and fantasizing: Thought flow and motivation. In K. D. MarkmanW. M. P. KleinJ. A. SuhrEds., Handbook of imagination and mental simulation (pp. 225–239). New York, NY: Psychology Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Klinger, E. & Cox, W. M. (2004). Motivation and the theory of current concerns. In W. M. CoxE. KlingerEds., Handbook of motivational counseling: Motivating people for change (pp. 3–23). Chichester, UK: Wiley. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Leary, M. R., Adams, C. E. & Tate, E. B. (2006). Hypo-egoic self-regulation: Exercising self-control by diminishing the influence of the self. Journal of Personality, 74, 1803–1831. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2006.00429.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lee, F. K., Sheldon, K. M. & Turban, D. B. (2003). Personality and the goal-striving process: The influence of achievement goal patterns, goal level, and mental focus on performance and enjoyment. The Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 256–265. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.2.256 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Leonard, N. R., Gwadz, M. V., Ritchie, A., Linick, J. L., Cleland, C. M., Elliott, L. & Grethel, M. (2015). A multi-method exploratory study of stress, coping, and substance use among high school youth in private schools. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1028. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01028 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lewin, T. (2011, January 26). Record level of stress in college freshmen. The New York Times. Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com/2011/01/27/education/27colleges.html First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Linnenbrink, E. A., Ryan, A. M. & Pintrich, P. R. (1999). The role of goals and affect in working memory functioning. Learning and Individual Differences, 11, 213–230. doi: 10.1016/S1041-6080(00)80006-0 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Luthar, S. S. & Becker, B. E. (2002). Privileged but pressured? A study of affluent youth. Child Development, 73, 1593–1610. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00492 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Maier, L. J., Liechti, M. E., Herzig, F. & Schaub, M. P. (2013). To dope or not to dope: Neuroenhancement with prescription drugs and drugs of abuse among Swiss university students. PLoS One, 8, e77967. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0077967 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Markman, A. B., Maddox, W. T. & Worthy, D. A. (2006). Choking and excelling under pressure. Psychological Science, 17, 944–948. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01809.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Matlack, C. (2013, June 21). France’s Baccalauréat: Not worth the pain. Businessweek. Retrieved from http://www.businessweek.com/articles/2013-06-21/frances-baccalaureat-not-worth-the-pain First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • McGregor, H. A. & Elliot, A. J. (2002). Achievement goals as predictors of achievement-relevant processes prior to task engagement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 381–395. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.94.2.381 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McVay, J. C. & Kane, M. J. (2010). Does mind wandering reflect executive function or executive failure? Comment on Smallwood and Schooler (2006) and Watkins (2008). Psychological Bulletin, 136, 188–197. doi: 10.1037/a0018298 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • A. MiyakeP. Shah (1999). Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Muller, D., Atzeni, T. & Butera, F. (2004). Coaction and upward social comparison reduce illusory conjunction effect: Some support for distraction-conflict theory. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 659–665. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2003.12.003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Muller, D. & Butera, F. (2007). The focusing effect of self-evaluation threat in coaction and social comparison. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 194–211. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.93.2.194 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Murayama, K. & Elliot, A. J. (2012). The competition-performance relation: A meta-analytic review and test of the opposing processes model of competition and performance. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 1035–1070. doi: 10.1037/a0028324 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nicholls, J. G. (1984). Achievement motivation: Conceptions of ability, subjective experience, task choice, and performance. Psychological Review, 91, 328–346. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.91.3.328 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • OECD. (2013). Education at a glance 2013: OECD Indicators, OECD Publishing. doi: 10.1787/eag-2013-en First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pekrun, R., Elliot, A. J. & Maier, M. A. (2009). Achievement goals and achievement emotions: Testing a model of their joint relations with academic performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 115–135. doi: 10.1037/a0013383 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pérez-Pena, R. (2014, April 8). Best, brightest, and rejected: Elite colleges turn away up to 95%. The New York Times. Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com/2014/04/09/us/led-by-stanfords-5-top-colleges-acceptance-rates-hit-new-lows.html?_r=0 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Pope, D. C. (2001). Doing school: How we are creating a generation of stressed-out, materialistic, and miseducated students. London, UK: Yale University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Poortvliet, P. M. & Darnon, C. (2013). Understanding Positive Attitudes toward Helping Peers: The Role of Mastery Goals and Academic Self-Efficacy. Self & Identity, 13, 345–363. doi: 10.1080/15298868.2013.832363 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Poortvliet, P. M., Janssen, O., Van Yperen, N. W. & Van de Vliert, E. (2007). Achievement goals and interpersonal behavior: How mastery and performance goals shape information exchange. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 1435–1447. doi: 10.1177/0146167207305536 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Poortvliet, P. M., Janssen, O., Van Yperen, N. W. & Van de Vliert, E. (2009). Low ranks make the difference: How achievement goals and ranking information affect cooperation intentions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 1144–1147. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2009.06.013 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pulfrey, C. & Butera, F. (2013). Why neoliberal values of self-enhancement lead to cheating in higher education: A motivational account. Psychological Science, 24, 2153–2162. doi: 10.1177/0956797613487221 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ryan, R. M. & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. The American Psychologist, 55, 68–78. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.68 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ryan, R. M. & Weinstein, N. (2009). Undermining quality teaching and learning: A self-determination theory perspective on high-stakes testing. Theory and Research in Education, 7, 224–233. doi: 10.1177/1477878509104327 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schwartz, A. (2012, June 9). Risky rise of the good-grade pill. The New York Times. Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com/2012/06/10/education/seeking-academic-edge-teenagers-abuse-stimulants.html First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Schwartz, A. (2012, June 9). In their own words: "Study drugs". The New York Times. Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com/interactive/2012/06/10/education/stimulants-student-voices.html#/#1 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Schwartz, S. H. (2007). Cultural and individual value correlates of capitalism: A comparative analysis. Psychological Inquiry, 18, 52–57. doi: 10.1080/10478400701388963 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Senko, C. (2016). Achievement goal theory: A story of early promises, eventual discords, and future possibilities. In K. R. WentzelD. B. MieleEds., Handbook of Motivation at School (pp. 75–95). New York, NY: Routledge. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Senko, C., Hulleman, C. S. & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2011). Achievement goal theory at the crossroads: Old controversies, current challenges, and new directions. Educational Psychologist, 46, 26–47. doi: 10.1080/00461520.2011.538646 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Senko, C., Hama, H. & Belmonte, K. (2013). Achievement goals, study strategies, and achievement. A test of the “learning agenda” framework. Learning and Individual Differences, 24, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2012.11.003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Senko, C. & Miles, K. M. (2008). Pursuing their own learning agenda: How mastery-oriented students jeopardize their class performance. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 33, 561–583. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2007.12.001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Skaalvik, E. M. (1997). Self-enhancing and self-defeating ego orientation: Relations with task and avoidance orientation, achievement, self-perceptions, and anxiety. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 71–81. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.89.1.71 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Urdan, T. C. & Maehr, M. L. (1995). Beyond a two-goal theory of motivation and achievement: A case for social goals. Review of Educational Research, 65, 213–243. doi: 10.3102/00346543065003213 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vallacher, R. R. & Wegner, D. M. (1989). Levels of personal agency: Individual variation in action identification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 660–671. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.57.4.660 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vansteenkiste, M., Lens, W., Elliot, A. J., Mouratidis, A. & Soenens, B. (2014). Moving the achievement goal approach one step forward: Toward a systematic examination of the autonomous and controlled reasons underlying achievement goals. Educational Psychologist, 49, 153–174. doi: 10.1080/00461520.2014.928598 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vansteenkiste, M., Matos, L., Lens, W. & Soenens, B. (2007). Understanding the impact of intrinsic versus extrinsic goal framing on exercise performance: The conflicting role of task and ego involvement. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 8, 771–794. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2006.04.006 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vansteenkiste, M., Smeets, S., Lens, W., Soenens, B., Matos, L. & Deci, E. L. (2010). Autonomous and controlled regulation of performance-approach goals: Their relations to perfectionism and educational outcomes. Motivation and Emotion, 34, 333–353. doi: 10.1007/s11031-010-9188-3 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Watkins, E. R. (2008). Constructive and unconstructive repetitive thought. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 163–206. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.163 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Review, 101, 34–52. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.101.1.34 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wegner, D. M. & Erber, R. (1992). The hyperaccessibility of suppressed thoughts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 903–912. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.63.6.903 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wigfield, A. & Eccles, J. S. (2000). Expectancy-value theory of achievement motivation. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 68–81. doi: 10.1006/ceps.1999.1015 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wolters, C. A., Yu, S. L. & Pintrich, P. R. (1996). The relation between goal orientation and students’ motivational beliefs and self-regulated learning. Learning and Individual Differences, 8, 211–238. doi: 10.1016/S1041-6080(96)90015-1 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar