Skip to main content
Original Article

Relating Individual Differences in Trait-Anxiety to Memory Functioning in Young Children

An Investigation With Task-Irrelevant Emotional Stimuli

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1614-0001/a000079

There is extensive evidence indicating cognitive biases at several stages of information processing in high-anxious children. Little research, however, has investigated a potential memory bias toward negative information in high-anxious young children. We studied immediate and delayed verbal recall as well as delayed visual recognition in a sample of high-trait-anxious (HA) and low-trait-anxious (LA) preschoolers (N = 76, mean age = 65 months), using stimuli containing task-irrelevant emotional valence (positive, negative, neutral). The findings revealed that, compared to their LA counterparts, HA preschoolers displayed (1) a tendency to be less accurate in the immediate verbal recall task, (2) poorer recall of negative words in the immediate condition and poorer recall of neutral words in the delayed condition, (3) impaired delayed recognition of identities with happy facial expressions and a tendency to better recognize identities expressing anger. Results are discussed considering the dynamic interplay between personality, emotion and cognitive factors during early development.

References

  • Alloway, T. P. , Gathercole, S. E. , Pickering, S. J. (2006). Verbal and visuo-spatial short term and working memory in children: Are they separable? Child Development, 77, 1698–1716. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Altarriba, J. , Bauer, L. M. (2004). The distinctiveness of emotion concepts: A comparison between emotion, abstract, and concrete words. American Journal of Psychology, 117, 389–410. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bar-Haim, Y. (2010). Research review: Attention bias modification (ABM): A novel treatment for anxiety disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 859–870. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Batty, M. , Taylor, M. J. (2006). The development of emotional face processing during childhood. Developmental Science, 9, 207–220. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baumeister, R. F. , Bratslavsky, E. , Finkenauer, C. , Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5, 323–370. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Benga, O. , Tincaş, I. , Visu-Petra, L. (2010). Investigating the structure of anxiety symptoms among Romanian preschoolers using the Spence Preschool Anxiety Scales. Cognition, Brain, Behavior, 14, 159–182. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bradley, M. M. , Lang, P. J. (1999). Affective norms for English words (ANEW): Stimuli, instruction manual and affective ratings. Technical report C-1. Gainesville, FL: The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Case, R. , Kurland, D. , Goldberg, J. (1982). Operational efficiency and the growth of short-term memory span. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 33, 386–404. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cochran, R. E. , Lee, F. J. , Chown, E. (2006). Modeling emotion: Arousal’s impact on memory. In Proceedings of the 28th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1133–1138). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Daleiden, E. L. (1998). Childhood anxiety and memory functioning: A comparison of systemic and processing accounts. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 68, 216–235. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Daleiden, E. L. , Vasey, M. W. (1997). An information-processing perspective on childhood anxiety. Clinical Psychology Review, 17, 407–429. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Darke, S. (1988). Anxiety and working memory capacity. Cognition and Emotion, 2, 145–154. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Derakshan, N. , Eysenck, M. W. (2009). Anxiety, processing efficiency and cognitive performance: New developments from attentional control theory. European Psychologist, 14, 168–176. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Derryberry, D. , Reed, M. A. (1996). Regulatory processes and the development of cognitive representations. Development and Psychopathology, 8(1), 215–234. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dilley, M. G. , Paivio, A. (1968). Pictures and words as stimulus and response items in paired-associate learning of young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 6, 231–240. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ekman, P. , Friesen, W. (1976). Pictures of facial affect. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Eysenck, M. W. , Derakshan, N. , Santos, R. , Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion, 7, 336–353. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Field, A. P. , Lester, K. J. (2010). Learning of Information processing biases in anxious children and adolescents. In J. Hadwin A. P. Field, Eds., Information processing biases and anxiety: A developmental perspective (pp. 253–278). Chichester, UK: Wiley. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Grossmann, T. , Johnson, M. H. , Vaish, A. , Hughes, D. , Quinque, D. , Stoneking, M. , Friederici, A. (2011). Genetic and neural dissociation of individual responses to emotional expressions in human infants. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 57–66. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Grossmann, T. , Striano, T. , Friederici, A. D. (2006). Crossmodal integration of emotional information from face and voice in the infant brain. Developmental Science, 9, 309–315. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hadwin, J. , Brogan, J. , Stevenson, J. (2005). State anxiety and working memory in children: A test of processing efficiency theory. Educational Psychology, 25, 379–393. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hadwin, J. A. , Field, A. P. (2010). An introduction to the study of information processing biases in childhood anxiety: Theoretical and methodological issues. In J. A. Hadwin A. P. Field, Eds., Information processing biases and anxiety: A developmental perspective (pp. 1–18). Chichester, UK: Wiley. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hadwin, J. A. , Garner, M. , Perez-Olivas, G. (2006). The development of information processing biases in childhood anxiety: A review and exploration of its origins in parenting. Clinical Psychology Review, 26, 876–894. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ingram, R. E. , Price, J. M. (2010). Understanding psychopathology: The role of vulnerability. In R. E. Ingram J. M. Price, Eds., Vulnerability to psychopathology. Risk across the lifespan (pp. 3–17). New York: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kleinsmith, L. J. , Kaplan, S. (1964). The interaction of arousal and recall interval in nonsense syllable paired associate learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67, 124–126. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Korkman, M. , Kirk, U. , Kemp, S. (1998). NEPSY: A developmental neuropsychological assessment. New York: The Psychological Corporation. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Lavy, E. , van den Hout, M. A. (1994). Cognitive avoidance and attentional bias: Causal relationships. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 18, 179–191. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McClure, E. B. (2000). A meta-analytic review of sex differences in facial expression processing and their development in infants, children, and adolescents. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 424–453. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mitte, K. (2008). Memory bias for threatening information in anxiety and anxiety disorders: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 886–911. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mogg, K. , Mathews, A. , Weinman, J. (1987). Memory bias in clinical anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96, 94–98. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Moser, J. S. , Huppert, J. D. , Duval, E. , Simons, R. F. (2008). Face processing biases in social anxiety: An electrophysiological study. Biological Psychology, 78, 93–103. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Muris, P. (2006). The pathogenesis of childhood anxiety disorders: Considerations from a developmental psychopathology perspective. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 30, 5–11. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Muris, P. , Meesters, C. , Smulders, L. , Mayer, B. (2005). Threat perception distortions and psychopathological symptoms in typically developing children. Infant and Child Development, 14, 273–285. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Paz-Alonso, P. , Larson, R. P. , Castelli, P. , Alley, D. , Goodman, G. S. (2009). Memory development: Emotion, stress, and trauma. In M. L. Courage N. Cowan, Eds., The development of memory in infancy and childhood (2nd ed., pp. 197–239). New York: Psychology Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Pine, D. S. (2007). Research review: A neuroscience framework for pediatric anxiety disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 631–648. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Reid, S.-C. , Salmon, K. , Lovibond, P. (2006). Cognitive biases in childhood anxiety, depression, and aggression: Are they pervasive or specific? Cognitive Therapy and Research, 30, 531–549. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Richards, A. , French, C. C. , Nash, G. , Hadwin, J. , Donnelly, N. (2007). A comparison of selective attention and interpretation processing biases in typically developing children high and low in self-report trait anxiety. Development and Psychopathology, 19, 481–495. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Riskind, J. H. (1997). Looming vulnerability to threat: A cognitive paradigm for anxiety. Behavior Research and Therapy, 35, 386–404. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schneider, W. , Kron, V. , Hünnerkopf, M. , & Krajewski, K. (2004). The development of young children’s memory strategies: First findings from the Würzburg Longitudinal Study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 88, 193–209. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schniering, C. A. , Hudson, J. L. , Rapee, R. M. (2000). Issues in the diagnosis and assessment of anxiety disorders in children and adolescents. Clinical Psychology Review, 20, 453–478. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sharot, T. , Phelps, E. A. (2004). How arousal modulates memory: Disentangling the effects of attention and retention. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 4, 294–306. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Silvia, P. J. , Allan, W. D. , Beauchamp, D. L. , Maschauer, E. L. , Workman, J. O. (2006). Biased recognition of happy facial expressions in social anxiety. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 25, 585–602. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Spence, S. H. , Rapee, R. , McDonald, C. , Ingram, M. (2001). The structure of anxiety symptoms among preschoolers. Behavior Research and Therapy, 39, 1293–1316. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Thibodeau, R. , Jorgensen, R. S. , Jonovich, S. J. (2008). Anger elicitation using affective pictures: An individual differences approach. Journal of Individual Differences, 29, 80–89. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Tottenham, N. , Tanaka, J. , Leon, A. C. , McCarry, T. , Nurse, M. , Hare, T. A. , ... Nelson, C. A. (2009). The NimStim set of facial expressions: Judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Research, 168, 242–249. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Unsworth, N. , Miller, J. D. , Lakey, C. E. , Young, D. L. , Meeks, J. T. , Campbell, W. K. , Goodie, A. S. (2009). Exploring the relations among executive functions, fluid intelligence, and personality. Journal of Individual Differences, 30, 194–200. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Visu-Petra, L. , Cheie, L. , Benga, O. (in preparation). A working memory training program using emotional stimuli: Applications for high-anxious children. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Visu-Petra, L. , Cheie, L. , Benga, O. , Alloway, T. P. (2011). Effects of trait anxiety on memory storage and updating in young children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 35(1), 38–47. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Visu-Petra, L. , Cheie, L. , Miu, A. C. (in press). Working memory and anxiety: Exploring the interplay of individual differences across development. In T. P. Alloway R. G. Alloway, Eds., Working memory: The new intelligence. (Frontiers in cognitive psychology series; series editors: Nelson Cowan and David Balota). New York: Psychology Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Visu-Petra, L. , Miclea, M. , Cheie, L. , Benga, O. (2009). Processing efficiency in preschoolers’ memory span: A longitudinal investigation of individual differences related to age and anxiety. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103, 30–48. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Visu-Petra, L. , Tincaş, I. , Cheie, L. , Benga, O. (2010). Anxiety and memory updating in young children: An investigation using emotional facial expressions. Cognition and Emotion, 24, 223–240. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Weems, C. F. (2008). Developmental trajectories of childhood anxiety: Identifying continuity and change in anxious emotion. Developmental Review, 28, 488–502. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Weems, C. F. , Watts, S. E. (2005). Cognitive models of childhood anxiety. In C. M. Velotis, (Ed.), Anxiety disorder research (pp. 205–232). Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Widen, S. C. , Russell, J. A. (2003). A closer look at preschoolers’ freely produced labels for facial expressions. Developmental Psychology, 39, 114–128. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Widen, S. C. , Russell, J. A. (2008). Children acquire emotion categories gradually. Cognitive Development, 23, 291–312. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zelazo, P. D. , Carlson, S. M. , Kesek, A. (2008). Development of executive function in childhood. In C. A. Nelson M. Luciana, Eds., Handbook of developmental cognitive neuroscience (2nd ed., pp. 553–574). Cambridge, MA: MIT. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar