Skip to main content
Original Article

Must We Suffer to Succeed?

When Anxiety Boosts Motivation and Performance

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1614-0001/a000228

Abstract. Why do some people work best under pressure? In two studies, we examined whether and how people use anxiety to motivate themselves. As predicted, clarity of feelings moderated the relationship between trait anxiety and the tendency to use this emotion as a source of motivation (i.e., anxiety motivation). Furthermore, anxiety motivation mediated the relationship between trait anxiety and outcomes – including academic achievement (Study 1) as well as persistence and job satisfaction (Study 2). These findings suggest that individuals who are clear about their feelings are more likely to thrive on anxiety and eustress and possibly use these to achieve their goals and find satisfaction at work.

References

  • Aarts, H., Custers, R. & Holland, R. W. (2007). The nonconscious cessation of goal pursuit: When goals and negative affect are coactivated. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 165–178. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.92.2.165 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Barrett, L. F. & Gross, J. J. (2001). Emotion representation and regulation: A process model of emotional intelligence. In T. MayneG. BonnanoEds., Emotion: Current issues and future directions (pp. 286–310). New York, NY: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Boden, M. T. & Thompson, R. J. (2017). Meta-analysis of the association between emotional clarity and attention to emotions. Emotion Review, 9, 79–85. doi: 10.1177/1754073915610640 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brislin, R. W. (1970). Back-translation for cross-cultural research. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 1, 185–216. doi: 10.1177/135910457000100301 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brown, S. P., Westbrook, R. A. & Challagalla, G. (2005). Good cope, bad cope: Adaptive and maladaptive coping strategies following a critical negative work event. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 792–798. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.90.4.792 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Byron, K., Khazanchi, S. & Nazarian, D. (2010). The relationship between stressors and creativity: A meta-analysis examining competing theoretical models. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95, 201–212. doi: 10.1037/a0017868 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Calvo, M. G. & Eysenck, M. W. (1996). Phonological working memory and reading in test anxiety. Memory, 4, 289–305. doi: 10.1080/096582196388960 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carver, C. S. & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions of positive and negative affect: A control-process view. Psychological Review, 97, 19–35. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.97.1.19 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cervone, D., Kopp, D. A., Schaumann, L. & Scott, W. D. (1994). Mood, self-efficacy, and performance standards: Lower moods induce higher standards for performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 499–512. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.67.3.499 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Clore, G. L., Schwarz, N. & Conway, M. (1994). Affective causes and consequences of social information processing. In R. S. Wyer Jr.T. K. SrullEds., Handbook of Social Cognition (pp. 323–418). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Coté, S. & Moskowitz, D. S. (1998). On the dynamic covariation between interpersonal behavior and affect: Prediction from neuroticism, extraversion, and agreeableness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1032–1046. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.75.4.1032 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1993). The evolving self. New York, NY: Harper Perennial. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • De Dreu, C. K., Baas, M. & Nijstad, B. A. (2008). Hedonic tone and activation level in the mood-creativity link: Toward a dual pathway to creativity model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 739–756. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.94.5.739 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Erber, R., Erber, M. W. & Poe, J. (2004). Mood regulation and decision making: Is irrational exuberance really a problem?. In I. BrocasJ. D. CarilloEds., The psychology of economic decisions (pp. 197–221). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Eysenck, M. & Calvo, M. G. (1992). Anxiety and performance: The processing efficiency theory. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 409–434. doi: 10.1080/02699939208409696 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R. & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion, 7, 336–353. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.336 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Feldman-Barrett, L. (2006). Solving the emotion paradox: Categorization and the experience of emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 20–46. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr1001_2 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Foo, M. D., Uy, M. & Baron, R. A. (2009). How do feelings influence effort? An empirical study of entrepreneurs’ affect and venture effort. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94, 1086–1094. doi: 10.1037/a0015599 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of General Psychology, 2, 300–319. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.300 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Frijda, N. H. (2007). The laws of emotion. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Gagné, M., Forest, J., Gilbert, M.-H., Aubé, C., Morin, E. & Malorni, A. (2010). The Motivation at Work Scale: Validation evidence in two languages. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 70, 628–646. doi: 10.1177/0013164409355698 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gohm, C. L., Corser, G. C. & Dalsky, D. J. (2005). Emotional intelligence under stress: Useful, unnecessary, or irrelevant? Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 1017–1028. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.03.018 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gross, J. J. & Jazaieri, H. (2014). Emotion, emotion regulation, and psychopathology: An affective science perspective. Clinical Psychological Science, 2, 387–401. doi: 10.1177/2167702614536164 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Harmon-Jones, E. & Allen, J. J. (1998). Anger and frontal brain activity: EEG asymmetry consistent with approach motivation despite negative affective valence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1310–1316. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.5.1310 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hayes, A. F. (2012). PROCESS: A versatile computational tool for observed variable mediation, moderation, and conditional process modeling [White paper]. Retrieved from http://www.afhayes.com/public/process2012.pdf First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hirt, E. R., McDonald, H. E. & Melton, R. J. (1996). Processing goals and the affect-performance link: Mood as main effect or mood as input?. In L. L. MartinA. TesserEds., Striving and feeling: Interactions among goals, affect, and self-regulation (pp. 303–328). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ilies, R. & Judge, T. A. (2005). Goal regulation across time: The effect of feedback and affect. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 453–467. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.90.3.453 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Izard, C. E. & Ackerman, B. P. (2000). Motivational, organizational, and regulatory functions of discrete emotions. In M. LewisJ. Haviland-JonesEds., Handbook of emotions (pp. 253–322). New York, NY: Guilford Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Izard, C. E., Woodburn, E. M., Finlon, K. J., Krauthamer-Ewing, E. S., Grossman, S. R. & Seidenfeld, A. (2011). Emotion knowledge, emotion utilization, and emotion regulation. Emotion Review, 3, 44–52. doi: 10.1177/1754073910380972 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Laux, L., Glanzmann, P., Schaffner, P. & Spielberger, C. D. (1981). State-Trait-Angst-Inventar STAI. Weinheim, Germany: Beltz. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Lawton, M. P. (2001). Emotion in later life. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10, 120–123. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00130 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Le Fevre, M., Matheny, J. & Kolt, G. S. (2003). Eustress, distress, and interpretation of occupational stress. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 18, 726–744. doi: 10.1108/02683940610684391 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Louro, M. J., Pieters, R. & Zeelenberg, M. (2007). Dynamics of multiple goal pursuit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 174–193. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.93.2.174 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Martens, R., Burton, D., Vealey, R. S., Bump, L. A. & Smith, D. E. (1990). Development and validation of the Competitive State Anxiety Inventory-2 (CSAI-2). In R. MartensR. S. VealeyD. BurtonEds., Competitive anxiety in sport (pp. 117–213). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Martin, L. L. & Stoner, P. (1996). Mood as input: What we think about how we feel determines how we think. In L. L. MartinA. TesserEds., Striving and feeling: Interactions among goals, affect, and self-regulation (pp. 279–301). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Martin, L. L., Ward, D. W., Achee, J. W. & Wyer, R. S. (1993). Mood as input: People have to interpret the motivational implications of their moods. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 317–326. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.64.3.317 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mayer, J. D. & Salovey, P. (1997). What is emotional intelligence?. In P. SaloveyD. SluyterEds., Emotional development and emotional intelligence: Implications for educators (pp. 3–31). New York, NY: Basic Books. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • McGonigal, K. (2015). The upside of stress: Why stress is good for you and how to get good at it. New York, NY: Avery. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Morris, L. W., Davis, M. A. & Hutchings, C. H. (1981). Cognitive and emotional components of anxiety: Literature review and a revised worry-emotionality scale. Journal of Educational Psychology, 73, 541–555. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.73.4.541 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Parrott, W. G. (1993). Beyond hedonism: Motives for inhibiting good moods and for maintaining bad moods. In D. M. WegnerJ. W. PennebakerEds., Handbook of mental control (pp. 278–305). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Parrott, W. G. (2002). The functional utility of negative emotions. In L. F. BarrettP. SaloveyEds., The wisdom in feeling: Psychological processes in emotional intelligence (pp. 341–359). New York, NY: Guilford Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Pearson, D. W. & Thackray, R. I. (1970). Consistency of performance change and autonomic response as a function of expressed attitude toward a specific stress situation. Psychophysiology, 6, 561–568. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1970.tb02245.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Titz, W. & Perry, R. P. (2002). Academic emotions in students’ self-regulated learning and achievement: A program of qualitative and quantitative research. Educational Psychologist, 37, 91–105. doi: 10.1207/S15326985EP3702_4 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Perkins, A. M. & Corr, P. J. (2005). Can worriers be winners? The association between worrying and job performance. Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 25–31. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2004.03.008 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Reynolds, W. M. (1982). Development of reliable and valid short forms of the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 38, 119–125. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(198201) First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1161–1178. doi: 10.1037/h0077714 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Russell, S. S., Spitzmüller, C., Lin, L. F., Stanton, J. M., Smith, P. C. & Ironson, G. H. (2004). Shorter can also be better: The abridged Job in General scale. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 64, 878–893. doi: 10.1177/0013164404264841 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Salovey, P., Mayer, J. D., Goldman, S. L., Turvey, C. & Palfai, T. P. (1995). Emotional attention, clarity, and repair: Exploring emotional intelligence using the trait meta-mood scale. In J. W. PennebakerEd., Emotion, disclosure, and health (pp. 125–154). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Selye, H. (1956). The stress of life. New York, NY: McGraw Hill. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Selye, H. (1987). Stress without distress. London, UK: Transworld, 197–221. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Siemsen, E., Roth, A. & Oliveira, P. (2010). Common method bias in regression models with linear, quadratic, and interaction effects. Organizational Research Methods, 13, 456–476. doi: 10.1177/1094428109351241 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Spielberger, C. D. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Stöber, J. (2004). Dimensions of test anxiety: Relations to ways of coping with pre-exam anxiety and uncertainty. Anxiety, Stress and Coping, 17, 213–226. doi: 10.1080/10615800412331292615 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Strack, J. & Esteves, F. (2015). Exams? Why worry? Interpreting anxiety as facilitative and stress appraisals. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 28, 205–214. doi: 10.1080/10615806.2014.931942 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Strack, J., Lopes, P. & Esteves, F. (2015). Will you thrive under pressure or burn out? Linking anxiety motivation and emotional exhaustion. Cognition and Emotion, 29, 578–591. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2014.922934 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tamir, M. (2005). Don’t worry, be happy? Neuroticism, trait-consistent affect regulation, and performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 449–461. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.89.3.449 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tamir, M., Chiu, C. Y. & Gross, J. J. (2007). Business or pleasure? Utilitarian versus hedonic considerations in emotion regulation. Emotion, 7, 546–554. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.3.546 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tamir, M. & Ford, B. Q. (2009). Choosing to be afraid: Preferences for fear as a function of goal pursuit. Emotion, 9, 488–497. doi: 10.1037/a0015882 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tamir, M., Mitchell, C. & Gross, J. J. (2008). Hedonic and instrumental motives in anger regulation. Psychological Science, 19, 324–328. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02088.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vagg, P. R., Spielberger, C. D. & O’Hearn, T. P. (1980). Is the state-trait anxiety inventory multidimensional? Personality and Individual Differences, 1, 207–214. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(80)90052-5 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wong, C. S. & Law, K. S. (2002). The effects of leader and follower emotional intelligence on performance and attitude: An exploratory study. Leadership Quarterly, 13, 243–274. doi: 10.1016/S1048-9843(02)00099-1 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wrosch, C., Heckhausen, J. & Lachman, M. E. (2000). Primary and secondary control strategies for managing health and financial stress across adulthood. Psychology and Aging, 15, 387–399. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.15.3.387 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar