Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169.49.1.3

Abstract. This paper presents an extension of the process-dissociation procedure with wordstem completion, which makes possible the measurement of the stochastic relationship between consciously controlled and automatic processes. By means of an indirect wordstem completion test, the conditional probabilities of conscious remembering with and without automatic processes can be successfully determined. A multinomial model for the evaluation of this extended process-dissociation procedure is presented. This model makes the distinction between voluntary and involuntary conscious memory processes possible and has been applied to two experiments discussed in this paper. The results show that the assumption of stochastic independence is often violated, albeit not as strongly as predicted by the redundancy or exclusivity model variants. Two conscious processes were found, voluntary and involuntary conscious memory processes, each with a different probability of occurrence.

References

  • Baddeley, A.(1997). Human memory: Theory and practice.. Erlbaum: Psychology Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Batchelder, W. H., Riefer, D. M.(1980). Separation of storage and retrieval factors in free recall of clusterable pairs. Psychological Review, 87, 375– 397 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Batchelder, W. H., Riefer, D. M.(1990). Multinomial processing models of source monitoring. Psychological Review, 97, 548– 564 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Batchelder, W. H., Riefer, D. M.(1999). Theoretical and empirical review of multinomial processing tree modeling. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 6, 57– 86 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Boddenberg, S.(2000). Kontrollierte und automatische Gedächtnisprozesse bzgl. traumarelevanter Reize bei PTSD-Patienten.. Unpublished thesis: Bonn. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Brainerd, C. J., Reyna, V. F., Mojardin, A. H.(1999). Conjoint recognition. Psychological Review, 106, 160– 179 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Bredenkamp, J., Vaterrodt-Plünnecke, B.(2001). Kontrollierte und automatische Gedächtnisprozesse bei PTSD-Patienten und Gesunden.. In R. Dohrenbusch, & F. A. Kaspers (Eds.), Fortschritte der Klinischen Psychologie und Verhaltensmedizin 89– 97 Lengerich: Pabst. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Buchner, A., Erdfelder, E., Vaterrodt-Plünnecke, B.(1995). Toward unbiased measurement of conscious and unconscious memory processes within the process-dissociation framework. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 137– 160 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Buchner, A., Erdfelder, E.(1996). On assumptions of, relations between, and evaluations of some process-dissociation measurement models. Consciousness and Cognition, 5, 581– 594 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Cohen, J.(1977). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. (rev. ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Cowan, N., Stadler, M. A.(1996). Estimating unconscious processes: Implications of a general class of models. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125, 195– 200 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Crowder, R. G., Morton, J.(1969). Precategorical acoustic storage (PAS). Perception and Psychophysics, 5, 365– 373 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Curran, T., Hintzman, D. L.(1995). Violations of the independence assumption in process-dissociation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 531– 547 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Debner, J. A., Jacoby, L. L.(1994). Unconscious perception: Attention, awareness, and control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 304– 417 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Ebbinghaus, H.(1964). Memory: A Contribution to experimental psychology. (H. A. Ruger, & C. E. Bussenius). New York: Dover Publications. (Original work published 1885). First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Engelkamp, J., Dehn, D.(1997). Strategy and consciousness in remembering subject-performed actions. Sprache und Kognition, 16, 94– 109 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A.(1998). Stochastic versus functional independence in the process-dissociation procedure. Unpublished manuscript. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Erdfelder, E., Faul, F., Buchner, A.(1996). GPOWER: A general power analysis program. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 28, 1– 11 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A.(1998). Process-dissociation measurement models: Threshold theory or detection theory?. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127, 83– 96 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Graf, P.(1995). Defining the opposition procedure: A reply to Reingold’s and Jacoby’s (1995) response to Graf and Komatsu (1994). European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 7, 225– 231 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Graf, P., Komatsu, S.-I.(1994). Process dissociation procedure: Handle with caution!. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 6, 113– 129 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hillstrom, A. P., Logan, G. D.(1997). Process dissociation, cognitive architecture, and response time: Comments on Lindsay and Jacoby (1994). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 1561– 1578 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Hintzman, D. L., Curran, T.(1997). More than one way to violate independence: Reply to Jacoby and Shrout (1997). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 511– 513 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hirshman, E.(1998). On the logic of testing the independence assumption in process-dissociation procedure. Memory & Cognition, 26, 857– 859 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Hu, X.(1991). Statistical inference program for multinomial binary tree models. [computer program]. Irvine, CA: University of California at Irvine. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hu, X., Batchelder, W. H.(1994). The statistical analysis of general processing tree models with the EM algorithm. Psychometrika, 59, 21– 48 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jacoby, L. L.(1991). A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 513– 541 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jacoby, L. L.(1998). Invariance in automatic influences of memory: Toward a user’s guide for the process-dissociation procedure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 3– 26 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Jacoby, L. L.(1999). Ironic effects of repetition: Measuring age-related differences in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 3– 22 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Jacoby, L. L., Begg, I. M., Toth, J. P.(1997). In defense of functional independence: Violations of assumptions underlying the process-dissociation procedure?. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 484– 495 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jacoby, L. L., Hay, J. F.(1998). Age-related deficits in memory: Theory and application.. In M. A. Conway, S. Gathercole & C. Cornoldi (Eds.), Theories of memory II. London: Psychology Press Ltd. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Jacoby, L. L., Shrout, P. E.(1997). Toward a psychometric analysis of violations of the independence assumption in process-dissociation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 505– 510 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jacoby, L. L., Toth, J. P., Yonelinas, A. P.(1993). Separating conscious and unconscious influences of memory: Measuring recollection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 139– 154 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jacoby, L. L., Toth, J. P., Yonelinas, A. P., Debner, J. A.(1994). The relationship between conscious and unconscious influences: Independence or redundancy?. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123, 216– 219 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jacoby, L. L., Yonelinas, A. P., Jennings, J. M.(1997). The relationship between conscious and unconscious (automatic) influences: A declaration of independence.. In J. Cohen & J. W. Schooler (Eds.) Scientific approaches to the question of consciousness, 13– 47 Hillsdale, NJ: LEA. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Jones, G. V.(1987). Independence and exclusivity among psychological processes: Implications for the structure of recall. Psychological Review, 94, 229– 235 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Joordens, S., Merikle, P. M.(1993). Independence or redundancy? Two models of conscious and unconscious influences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 462– 467 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Krüger, T.(1996). Untersuchungen zur Beziehung bewußter und unbewußter Gedächtnisprozesse. [Investigations on the relation between conscious and unconscious memory processes]. (Thesis) Berlin: Logos. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Krüger, T.(1998). Eine Normierung der Ergänzungen deutscher Wortanfänge zu Substantiven mit fünf oder sechs Buchstaben [Normative values for the completion of German wordstems]. Sprache & Kognition, 17, 51– 72 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Krüger, T.(1999). Die Erfassung bewußter und unbewußter Gedächtnisprozesse: Die Prozeß-Dissoziations-Prozedur. Probleme und Perspektiven einer neuen Methode.. [The measurement of conscious and unconscious memory processes: The process-dissociation procedure. Problems and perspectives of a new method]. Lengerich: Pabst. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Krüger, T., Vaterrodt-Plünnecke, B.(1997). Die stochastische Beziehung bewußter und automatischer Prozesse: Eine Erweiterung der Prozeß-Dissoziations-Prozedur [The stochastic relation between conscious and automatic processes: An extension of the process-dissociation procedure]. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 44, 220– 245 First citation in articleMedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Krüger, T., Vaterrodt-Plünnecke, B., Bredenkamp, J.(1997). Prozeß-Dissoziations-Modelle: Kritik, Erweiterung und Überprüfung. [Process-dissociation procedure: Critics, extensions, and tests].. In H. Mandl (Ed.), Bericht über den 40.Kongreß der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Psychologie in München 1996 (pp. 284– 289 Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Lindsay, D. S., Jacoby, L. L.(1994). Stroop process-dissociations: The relationship between facilitation and interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 219– 234 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Lindsay, D. S., Kelley, C. M.(1996). Creating illusions of familiarity in a cued recall remember/know paradigm. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 197– 211 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mecklenbräuker, S., Wippich, W., Mohrhusen, S. H.(1997). Conscious and unconscious influences of memory in a conceptual task: Limitations of a process-dissociation procedure.. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 55, 34– 48 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Morris, C. D., Bransford, J. D., Franks, J. J.(1977). Levels of processing versus transfer appropriate processing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, 519– 533 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Read, T. R. C., Cressie, N. A. C.(1988). Goodness-of-fit statistics for discrete multivariate data.. New York: Springer. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Richardson-Klavehn, A., Bjork, R. A.(1988). Measures of memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 39, 475– 543 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Richardson-Klavehn, A., Gardiner, J. M.(1995). Retrieval volition and memorial awareness in stem completion: An empirical analysis. Psychological Research, 57, 166– 178 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Richardson-Klavehn, A., Gardiner, J. M., Java, R. I.(1994). Involuntary conscious memory and the method of opposition. Memory, 2, 1– 29 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Richardson-Klavehn, A., Gardiner, J. M., Java, R. I.(1996). Memory: Task dissociation, process-dissociation and dissociations of consciousness.. In G. Underwood (Ed.) Implicit cognition., 85– 158 Oxford: University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Richardson-Klavehn, A., Gardiner, J. M.(1998). Depth-of processing effects on priming in stem completion: Tests of the voluntary-contamination, conceptual-processing, and lexical-processing hypotheses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 593– 609 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Riefer, D. M., Batchelder, W. H.(1988). Multinomial modeling and the measurement of cognitive processes. Psychological Review, 95, 318– 339 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Riefer, D. M., Batchelder, W. H.(1991). Statistical inference for multinomial processing tree models.. In J.-P. Doignon, & J.-P. Falmagne (Ed.) Mathematical psychology. Current Developments, 313– 335 New York: Springer. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Russo, R., Andrade, J.(1995). The directed forgetting effect in word fragment completion: An application of the process dissociation procedure. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48A, 405– 423 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Russo, R., Cullis, A. M., Parkin, A. J.(1998). Consequences of violating the assumption of independence in a process dissociation procedure: A word fragment completion study. Memory & Cognition, 26, 617– 632 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Schacter, D. L.(1987). Implicit memory: History and current status. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 501– 518 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Toth, J. P., Reingold, E. M., Jacoby, L. L.(1994). Toward a redefinition of implicit memory: Process-dissociations following elaborative processing and self-generation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 290– 303 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Vaterrodt-Plünnecke, B.(1994). Multinomiale Modellierung impliziter Gedächtnisprozesse: Ein alternativer Ansatz.[Multinomial modeling of implicit memory processes: An alternative approach]. Zeitschrift für experimentelle und angewandte Psychologie, 41, 295– 314 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Wippich, W.(1994). Unbewußte Effekte und Voreingenommenheiten bei Urteilen zu Personennamen [Unconscious effects and preconceptions in judgments of person names]. Zeitschrift für experimentelle und angewandte Psychologie, 41, 151– 172 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Yonelinas, A. P., Jacoby, L. L.(1995). Dissociating automatic and controlled processes in a memory-search task: Beyond implicit memory. Psychological Research, 57, 156– 165 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Yonelinas, A. P., Jacoby, L. L.(1996). Response bias and the process-dissociation procedure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125, 422– 439 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar