Skip to main content
Original Article

The Complicated Relationship Between Media and Morality

A Response to Ron Tamborini’s Model of “Moral Intuition and Media Entertainment,” From a Narrative Perspective

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-1105/a000032

This contribution reflects content and effects of moral messages in media from a narrative perspective. Building on Tamborini’s (2011) Model of Moral Intuition and Media Enjoyment, several issues are raised: First, the difficulty of conceptually and empirically defining “morality” in media entertainment is elaborated. Several options of moral ambiguity arising from the narrative structure of a story are sketched. Then, the link between processing moral content and positive media experiences such as enjoyment and appreciation is considered. Finally, issues of effects, especially on the long-term, are raised and implications for empirical questions are discussed.

References

  • Bilandzic, H. , Busselle, R. W. (2008). Transportation and transportability in the cultivation of genre-consistent attitudes and estimates. Journal of Communication, 58, 508–529. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bilandzic, H. , Hastall, M. (in press). Talking about Big Brother: Interpersonal communication about a controversial television format. In A. Hetsroni, (Ed.), Reality television: Merging the global and the local. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bilandzic, H. , Sukalla, F. , & Kinnebrock, S. (2008). Die Moral von der Geschichte: Methodische Probleme und Methodenentwicklung in der Medienanalyse [The Moral of the Story: Methodological problems and method development for media analysis]. In J. v. Gottberg, E. Prommer, (Eds.), Verlorene Werte? Medien und die Entwicklung von Ethik und Moral (pp. 245–256). Konstanz: UVK-Verlag. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bruner, J. (1991). The narrative construction of reality. Critical Inquiry, 18(1), 1–21. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Busselle, R. W. , Bilandzic, H. (2008). Fictionality and perceived realism in experiencing stories: A model of narrative comprehension and engagement. Communication Theory, 18, 255–280. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Busselle, R. W. , Bilandzic, H. (2009). Measuring narrative engagement. Media Psychology, 12, 321–347. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carroll, N. (1998). Art, narrative, and moral understanding. In J. Levinson, (Ed.), Aesthetics and ethics: Essays at the intersection (pp. 126–160). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chatman, S. B. (1989). Story and discourse: Narrative structure in fiction and film (5th ed.). Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Connolly, T. , Hardman, D. (2009). “Fools rush in”: A JDM perspective on the role of emotions in decisions, moral and otherwise. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 50, 275–306. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Damasio, A. R. (1999). The feeling of what happens. New York, NY: Grosset/Putnam. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Green, M. C. , Brock, T. C. (2000). The role of transportation in the persuasiveness of public narratives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 701–721. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Green, M. C. , Brock, T. C. (2002). In the mind’s eye: Transportation-imagery model of narrative persuasion. In M. C. Green, J. J. Strange, T. C. Brock, (Eds.), Narrative impact: Social and cognitive foundations (pp. 315–341). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108, 814–834. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Haidt, J. , Joseph, C. (2008). The moral mind: How five sets of innate intuitions guide the development f many culture-specific virtues, and perhaps even modules. In P. Carruthers, S. Laurence, S. Stich, (Eds.), The innate mind: Vol. 3. Foundations and the future (Evolution and Cognition) (pp. 367–392). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hakemulder, J. (2000). The moral laboratory: Experiments examining the effects of reading literature on social perception and moral self-concept. Amsterdam: Benjamins. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hakemulder, J. (2008). Literary potential: The unexplored powers of reading. Information Design Journal, 16, 126–132. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kohlberg, L. (1981). The philosophy of moral development: Moral stages and the idea of justice (Essays on moral development, Volume 1). San Francisco, CA: Harper & Row. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Oliver, M. B. , Bartsch, A. (in press). Appreciation as audience response: Exploring entertainment gratifications beyond hedonism. Human Communication Research. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Shrum, L. J. , Burroughs, J. E. , Rindfleisch, A. (2004). A process model of consumer cultivation: The role of television is a function of the type of judgment. In L. J. Shrum, (Ed.), The psychology of entertainment media: blurring the lines between entertainment and persuasion (pp. 177–191). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Sloman, S. A. , Fernbach, P. M. , Ewing, S. (2009). Causal models: The representational infrastructure for moral judgment. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 50, 1–26. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tamborini, R. (2011). Moral intuition and media entertainment. Journal of Media Psychology, 23, 39–45. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Van den Bulck, J. (2003). The datasetting effect of learning fact from fiction: Toward an on-line processing model of television effects. Paper presented at the Annual Conference of the International Communication Association, San Diego, CA. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar