Skip to main content
Original Article

An Investigation of Prospective and Retrospective Coding in Capuchin Monkeys and Rhesus Monkeys

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/a000052

The purpose of the present experiment was to investigate prospective and retrospective memory-coding strategies employed by rhesus monkeys and capuchin monkeys. Subjects were presented with a computerized radial arm maze analog. Monkeys used a joystick to move a cursor to touch identical stimuli spread around the screen. Once the monkeys were proficient at visiting each stimulus once, a delay was inserted into the trial (after 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, or 7 stimuli had been touched). Following the delay, monkeys were presented with a forced choice of two stimuli, one that had not yet been touched (correct) and one that had been touched (incorrect). The number of correct responses made after each delay was assessed. With the exception of one rhesus monkey, the animals did not appear to use prospective memory and instead relied heavily on retrospective memory. This result is in contrast to the previously reported results with rats, pigeons, and humans.

References

  • Basile, B. M. , Hampton, R. R. , Suomi, S. J. , Murray, E. A. (2009). An assessment of memory awareness in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Animal Cognition, 12, 169–180. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cook, R. G. , Brown, M. F. , Riley, D. A. (1985). Flexible memory processing by rats: Use of prospective and retrospective information in the radial maze. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 11, 453–469. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • DiGian, K. A. , Zentall, T. R. (2007). Pigeons may not use dual-coding in the radial maze analog task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 33, 262–272. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Einstein, G. O. , McDaniel, M. A. (1990). Normal aging and prospective memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 716–726. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Einstein, G. O. , McDaniel, M. A. (2005). Prospective memory: Multiple retrieval processes. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 286–290. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ellis, J. (1996). Prospective memory or the realization of delayed intentions: A conceptual framework for research. In M. Brandimonte, G. O. Einstein, M. A. McDaniel, (Eds.), Prospective memory: Theory and applications (pp. 1–22). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ellis, J. A. , Freeman, J. E. (2008). Realizing delayed intentions. In M. Kliegel, M. A. McDaniel, G. O. Einstein, (Eds.), Prospective memory: Cognitive, neuroscience, developmental and applied perspectives (pp. 1–27). New York, NY: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Evans, T. A. , Beran, M. J. , Chan, B. , Klein, E. D. , Menzel, C. R. (2008). An efficient computerized testing method for the capuchin monkey (Cebus apella): Adaptation of the LRC-CTS to a socially housed nonhuman primate species. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 590–596. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Flemming, T. J. , Beran, M. J. , Washburn, D. A. (2007). Disconnect in concept learning by rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta): Judgment of relations and relations-between-relations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 33, 55–63. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Grant, D. S. (1981). Short-term memory in the pigeon. In N. E. Spear, R. R. Miller, (Eds.), Information processing in animals: Memory mechanisms. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hoffman, M. L. , Beran, M. J. , Washburn, D. A. (2009). Memory for “what”, “where”, and “when” information in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 35, 143–152. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Honig, W. K. (1978). Studies of working memory in the pigeon. In S. H. Hulse, H. Fowler, W. W. Honig, (Eds.), Cognitive processes in animal behavior (pp. 211–248). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Jackson-Smith, P. , Zentall, T. R. , & Steirn, J. N. (1993). Prospective and retrospective memory processes in pigeons’ performance on a successive delayed matching-to-sample task. Learning & Motivation, 24, 1–22. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Janson, C. H. (2007). Experimental evidence for route integration and strategic planning in wild capuchin monkeys. Animal Cognition, 10, 341–356. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kennedy, E. H. , Fragaszy, D. H. (2008). Analogical reasoning in a capuchin monkey (Cebus apella). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 122, 167–175. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kesner, R. P. , DeSpain, M. J. (1988). Correspondence between rats and humans in utilization of retrospective and prospective codes. Animal Learning & Behavior, 16, 299–302. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kliegel, M. , Mackinlay, R. , Jager, T. (2008). Complex prospective memory: Development across the lifespan and the role of task interruption. Developmental Psychology, 44, 612–617. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kliegel, M. , McDaniel, M. A. , Einstein, G. O. (2000). Plan formation, retention, and execution in prospective memory: A new approach and age related effects. Memory & Cognition, 28, 1041–1049. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kornell, N. (2009). Metacognition in humans and animals. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 11–15. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kraemer, P. J. , Roberts, W. A. (1984). Short-term memory for visual and auditory stimuli in pigeons. Animal Learning & Behavior, 12, 275–284. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McDaniel, M. A. , Einstein, G. O. , Graham, T. , Rall, E. (2004). Delaying execution of intentions: Overcoming the costs of interruptions. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, 533–547. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Raby, C. R. , Clayton, N. S. (2009). Prospective cognition in animals. Behavioural Processes, 80, 314–324. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Richardson, W. K. , Washburn, D. A. , Hopkins, W. D. , Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. , Rumbaugh, D. M. (1990). The NASA/LRC computerized test system. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 22, 127–131. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Riley, D. A. , Cook, R. G. , & Lamb, M. R. (1981). A classification and analysis of short-term retention codes in pigeons. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 15, 51–79. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Saito, N. , Mushiake, H. , Sakamoto, K. , Itoyama, Y. , & Tanji, J. (2005). Representation of immediate and final behavioral goals in the monkey prefrontal cortex during an instructed delay period. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1535–1546. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Smith, J. D. (2009). The study of animal metacognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 389–396. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Smith, R. E. (2003). The cost of remembering to remember in event-based prospective memory: Investigating the capacity demands of delayed intention performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 347–361. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Smith, R. E. (2008). Attention, memory, and delayed intentions. In M. Kliegel, M. A. McDaniel, G. O. Einstein, (Eds.), Prospective memory: Cognitive, neuroscience, developmental and applied perspectives (pp. 29–52). New York, NY: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Smith, J. D. , Redford, J. S. , Beran, M. J. , Washburn, D. A. (2010). Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) adaptively monitor uncertainty while multi-tasking. Animal Cognition, 13, 93–101. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stoet, G. , Snyder, L. H. (2003). Task preparation in macaque monkeys (Macca mulatta). Animal Cognition, 6, 121–130. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Urcuioli, P. J. , Zentall, T. R. (1992). Transfer across delayed discriminations: Evidence regarding the nature of prospective working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 18, 154–173. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zentall, T. R. , Jagielo, J. A. , Jackson-Smith, P. , Urcuioli, P. J. (1987). Memory codes in pigeons’ short-term memory: Effects of varying the number of sample and comparison stimuli. Learning & Motivation, 18, 21–33. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zentall, T. R. , Steirn, J. N. , Jackson-Smith, P. (1990). Memory strategies in pigeons’ performance of a radial-arm-maze analog task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 16, 358–371. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar