Skip to main content
Short Research Note

Frustration and Violence in Mobile Video Games

An Experimental Evaluation of Their Effect on Implicit Aggression

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1024/1421-0185/a000236

Abstract. This study tests two competing hypotheses, one based on the general aggression model (GAM), the other on the self-determination theory (SDT). GAM suggests that the crucial factor in video games leading to increased aggressiveness is their violent content; SDT contends that gaming is associated with aggression because of the frustration of basic psychological needs. We used a 2×2 between-subject experimental design with a sample of 128 undergraduates. We assigned each participant randomly to one experimental condition defined by a particular video game, using four mobile video games differing in the degree of violence and in the level of their frustration-invoking gameplay. Aggressiveness was measured using the implicit association test (IAT), administered before and after the playing of a video game. We found no evidence of an association between implicit aggressiveness and violent content or frustrating gameplay.

References

  • Adachi, P. J. C., & Willoughby, T. (2011). The effect of video game competition and violence on aggressive behavior: Which characteristic has the greatest influence? Psychology of Violence, 1, 259–274. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024908 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Anderson, C. A., Carnagey, N. L., Flanagan, M., Benjamin, A. J., Eubanks, J., & Valentine, J. C. (2004). Violent video games: Specific effects of violent content on aggressive thoughts and behavior. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 36, 199–249. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(04)36004-1 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Anderson, C. A., & Dill, K. E. (2000). Video games and aggressive thoughts, feelings, and behavior in the laboratory and in life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 772–790. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.78.4.772 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Anderson, C. A., Shibuya, A., Ihori, N., Swing, E. L., Bushman, B. J., Sakamoto, A., … Saleem, M. (2010). Violent video game effects on aggression, empathy, and prosocial behavior in eastern and western countries: a meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 151–173. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018251 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Babchishin, K. M., Nunes, K. L., & Hermann, C. A. (2013). The validity of implicit association test (IAT) measures of sexual attraction to children: A meta-analysis. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 42, 487–499. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-012-0022-8 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Banse, R., Messer, M., & Fischer, I. (2015). Predicting aggressive behavior with the aggressiveness-IAT. Aggressive Behavior, 41, 65–83. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21574 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Banse, R., Seise, J., & Zerbes, N. (2001). Implicit attitudes towards homosexuality: Reliability, validity, and controllability of the IAT. Experimental Psychology, 48, 145–160. https://doi.org/10.1026//0949-3946.48.2.145 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Bender, J., Rothmund, T., & Gollwitzer, M. (2013). Biased estimation of violent video game effects on aggression: Contributing factors and boundary conditions. Societies, 3, 383–398. https://doi.org/10.3390/soc3040383 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bluemke, M., Friedrich, M., & Zumbach, J. (2009). The influence of violent and nonviolent computer games on implicit measures of aggressiveness. Aggressive Behavior, 35, 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.20329 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Buss, A. H., & Perry, M. (1992). The aggression questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 452–459. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.63.3.452 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cordeiro, P., Paixão, P., Lens, W., Lacante, M., & Luyckx, K. (2016). The Portuguese validation of the Basic Psychological Need Satisfaction and Frustration Scale: Concurrent and longitudinal relations to well-being and ill-being. Psychologica Belgica, 56, 193–209. https://doi.org/10.5334/pb.252 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Degner, J., Wentura, D., & Rothermund, K. (2006). Indirect assessment of attitudes with response-time-based measures. Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie, 37, 131–139. https://doi.org/10.1024/0044-3514.37.3.131 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Elson, M., & Ferguson, C. J. (2014). Twenty-five years of research on violence in digital games and aggression. European Psychologist, 19, 33–46. https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040/a000164 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Ferguson, C. J. (2015). Do Angry Birds make for angry children? A meta-analysis of video game influences on children’s and adolescents’ aggression, mental health, prosocial behavior, and academic performance. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10, 646–666. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615592234 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ferguson, C. J., & Wang, J. C. K. (2019). Aggressive video games are not a risk factor for future aggression in youth: A longitudinal study. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 48, 1439–1451. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-019-01069-0 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Furuya-Kanamori, L., & Doi, S. A. R. (2016). Angry Birds, angry children, and angry meta-analysts: A reanalysis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11, 408–414. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691616635599 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gawronski, B., & Hahn, A. (2019). Implicit measures: Procedure, use, and interpretation. In H. BlantonJ. M. LaCroixG. D. WebsterEds., Measurement in social psychology (pp. 29–55). New York, NY: Taylor & Francis. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Gollwitzer, M., Banse, R., Eisenbach, K., & Naumann, A. (2007). Effectiveness of the Vienna social competence training on explicit and implicit aggression: Evidence from an aggressiveness-IAT. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 23, 150–156. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759.23.3.150 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 197–216. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.197 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A. G., Poehlman, T. A., Uhlmann, E. L., & Banaji, M. R. (2009). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 17–41. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015575 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greitemeyer, T., & Mügge, D. O. (2014). Video games do affect social outcomes: A meta-analytic review of the effects of violent and prosocial video gameplay. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40, 578–589. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167213520459 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gunter, B. (2016). Does playing video games make players more violent?. London, UK: Palgrave Macmillan. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hollingdale, J., & Greitemeyer, T. (2014). The effect of online violent video games on levels of aggression. PLoS One, 9(11), e111790. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0111790 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ivory, D. H., Ivory, J. D., & Lanier, M. (2017). Video game use as risk exposure, protective incapacitation, or inconsequential activity among university students: Comparing approaches in a unique risk environment. Journal of Media Psychology, 29, 42–53. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-1105/a000210 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Kneer, J., Elson, M., & Knapp, F. (2016). Fight fire with rainbows: The effects of displayed violence, difficulty, and performance in digital games on affect, aggression, and physiological arousal. Computers in Human Behavior, 54, 142–148. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.07.034 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kuzucu, Y., & Şimşek, Ö. F. (2013). Self-determined choices and consequences: The relationship between basic psychological needs satisfactions and aggression in late adolescents. Journal of General Psychology, 140, 110–129. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.2013.771607 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lemmer, G., Gollwitzer, M., & Banse, R. (2015). On the psychometric properties of the aggressiveness-IAT for children and adolescents. Aggressive Behavior, 41, 84–95. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21575 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McCarthy, R. J., Coley, S. L., Wagner, M. F., Zengel, B., & Basham, A. (2016). Does playing video games with violent content temporarily increase aggressive inclinations? A preregistered experimental study. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 67, 13–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2015.10.009 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Millisecond Test Library. (2019). Retrieved from https://www.millisecond.com/download/library/iat/aggressioniat/ First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Nie, Y., Chua, B. L., Yeung, A. S., Ryan, R. M., & Chan, W. Y. (2015). The importance of autonomy support and the mediating role of work motivation for well-being: Testing self-determination theory in a Chinese work organisation. International Journal of Psychology, 50, 245–255. https://doi.org/10.1002/ijop.12110 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Przybylski, A. K., Rigby, C. S., Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2014). Competence-impeding electronic games and players’ aggressive feelings, thoughts, and behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106, 441–457. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034820 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Przybylski, A. K., & Weinstein, N. (2019). Violent video game engagement is not associated with adolescents’ aggressive behaviour: Evidence from a registered report. Royal Society Open Science, 6, 171474. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.171474 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ritter, D., & Eslea, M. (2005). Hot sauce, toy guns, and graffiti: A critical account of current laboratory aggression paradigms. Aggressive Behavior, 31, 407–419. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.20066 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2017). Self-determination theory: Basic psychological needs in motivation, development, and wellness. New York, NY: Guilford. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Savage, M. W., & Tokunaga, R. S. (2017). Moving toward a theory: Testing an integrated model of cyberbullying perpetration, aggression, social skills, and Internet self-efficacy. Computers in Human Behavior, 71, 353–361. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.02.016 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schnabel, K., Asendorpf, J. B., & Greenwald, A. G. (2008). Assessment of individual differences in implicit cognition. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 24, 210–217. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759.24.4.210 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Soto, C. J., & John, O. P. (2017). The next Big Five Inventory (BFI-2): Developing and assessing a hierarchical model with 15 facets to enhance bandwidth, fidelity, and predictive power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 113, 117–143. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000096 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Uhlmann, E., & Swanson, J. (2004). Exposure to violent video games increases automatic aggressiveness. Journal of Adolescence, 27, 41–52. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2003.10.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zumbach, J., Seitz, C., & Bluemke, M. (2015). Impact of violent video game realism on the self-concept of aggressiveness assessed with explicit and implicit measures. Computers in Human Behavior, 53, 278–288. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.07.018 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar