Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1026/0033-3042/a000086

Zusammenfassung. Bereits die antike Philosophie hat sich mit der Frage beschäftigt, warum Menschen zuweilen wider besseres Wissen den kurzfristigen Verlockungen des Augenblicks nachgeben und damit wichtigen langfristigen Zielen zuwider handeln. In der modernen Psychologie wird das Problem der Selbstkontrolle in einer ganzen Reihe von Ansätzen aufgegriffen, darunter kybernetische Modelle, intertemporale Entscheidungsmodelle, Zielintentions-Modelle, Zielkonflikt-Modelle, und Zweisystem-Modelle. Zweisystem-Modelle scheinen sich von den übrigen Ansätzen dadurch abzuheben, dass sie impulsive Prozesse der Verhaltensdetermination explizit thematisieren und in Erklärungsmodelle menschlichen Verhaltens integrieren. Wir berichten jüngere empirische Befunde, die aus einem solchen Ansatz erwachsen sind und diskutieren die Vorteile und Grenzen einer Zweisystem-Modellierung von Selbstkontrollkonflikten aus grundlagen- und anwendungsorientierter Sicht.


Two souls live in my breast. Psychological and philosophical considerations concerning the conflict between impulses and self-control

Abstract. Ancient philosophers already wondered why people sometimes act against their better judgment. That is, why do people yield to immediate short-term temptations even though they know that it will be detrimental to the pursuit of their long-term goals? Modern psychology has tackled the self-control problem from a variety of perspectives, such as cybernetic models, models of inter-temporal choice, goal-intention models, goal-conflict models, and dual-system models. One advantage of dual-system models seems to be that these models explicitly integrate impulsive processes of behavior determination into their theoretical architecture. We report a program of empirical research which has emanated from a dual-system approach and discuss the benefits and limitations of such an approach for basic and applied research.

Literatur

  • Aarts, H. , Dijksterhuis, A. , De Vries, P. (2001). On the psychology of drinking: Being thirsty and perceptually ready. British Journal of Psychology, 92, 631. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Aarts, H. , Verplanken, B. , van Knippenberg, A. (1998). Predicting behavior from actions in the past: Repeated decision making or a matter of habit? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28, 1356–1375. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ainslie, G. (1975). Specious reward: A behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control. Psychological Bulletin, 82, 463. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ainslie, G. (2001). Breakdown of will. New York: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50, 179–211. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ajzen, I. , Madden, T. J. (1986). Prediction of goal-directed behavior: Attitudes, intentions, and perceived behavioral control. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 22, 453–474. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ariely, D. , Loewenstein, G. (2006). The Heat of the Moment: The Effect of Sexual Arousal on Sexual Decision Making. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 19, 87–98. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Armitage, C. J. , Conner, M. (2001). Efficacy of the theory of planned behavior: A meta-analytic review. British Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 471–499. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Aron, A. R. (2008). Progress in executive-function research: From tasks to functions to regions to networks. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 124–129. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baddeley, A. D. (2007). Working memory, thought, and action. Oxford: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baumeister, R. F. , Bratslavsky, M. , Muraven, M. , Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1252–1265. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baumeister, R. F. , Vohs, K. D. (2004). Handbook of self-regulation. Reserach, theory, and applications. New York: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bechara, A. (2005). Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: A neurocognitive perspective. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1458–1463. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bechara, A. , Noel, X. , Crone, E. A. (2006). Loss of willpower: Abnormal neural mechanisms of impulse control and decision making in addiction. In R. W. Wiers, A. W. Stacy (Eds.), Handbook of implicit cognition and addiction (pp. 215–232). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Berridge, K. C. (2003). Pleasures of the brain. Brain and Cognition, 52, 106–128. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bertrams, A. , Dickhäuser, O. (2009). Messung dispositioneller Selbstkontroll-Kapazität: Eine deutsche Adaptation der Kurzform der Self-Control Scale (SCS-K-D). Diagnostica, 55, 2–10. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Block, N. (1995). On a confusion about a function of consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18, 227–287. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bobonich, C. , Destrée, P. (Eds.). (2007). Akrasia in Greek Philosophy. From Socrates to Plotinus. Leiden/Boston: Brill. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bogg, T. , Roberts, B. W. (2004). Conscientiousness and health-related behaviors: A meta-analysis of the leading behavioral contributors to mortality. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 887–919. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Boon, B. , Stroebe, W. , Shut, H. , Ijntema, R. (2002). Ironic processes in the eating behavior of restrained eaters. British Journal of Health Psychology, 7, 1–10. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Botvinick, M. M. , Braver, T. S. , Carter, C. S. , Barch, D. M. , Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108, 624–652. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cacioppo, J. T. , Petty, R. E. , Kao, C. F. (1984). The efficient assessment of need for cognition. Journal of Personality Assessment, 48, 306–307. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carver, C. S. (2005). Impulse and constraint: Perspectives from personality psychology, convergence with theory in other areas, and potential for integration. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9, 312–333. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carver, C. S. , Scheier, M. F. (1981). Attention and self-regulation: A control-theory approach to human behavior. New York: Springer. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carver, C. S. , Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions of positive and negative affect: A control-process view. Psychological Review, 97, 19–35. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cervone, D. , Shadel, W. G. , Smith, R. E. , Fiori, M. (2006). Self-regulation Reminders and suggestions from personality science. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 55, 333–385. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Collins, J. E. (1978). Effects of restraint, monitoring, and stimulus salience on eating behavior. Addictive Behaviors, 3, 197–204. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Conner, M. , Perugini, M. , O’Gorman , Ayres, K. , Prestwich, A. (2007). Relations between implicit and explicit measures of attitudes and measures of behavior: Evidence of moderation by individual difference variables. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 1727–1740. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Conrey, F. R. , Sherman, J. W. , Gawronski, B. , Hugenberg, K. , Groom, C. J. (2005). Separating Multiple Processes in Im-plicit Social Cognition: The Quad Model of Implicit Task Performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 469. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Davidson, D. (1980). How is weakness of will possible? In J. Feinberg (Ed.), Moral concepts (pp. 43–72). Oxford: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Davidson, D. (1982). Paradoxes of Irrationality. In R. Wollheim, J. Hopkins (Eds.), Philosophical Essays on Freud (pp. 289–305). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Davidson, D. (1985). Deception and Division. In J. Elster (Ed.), The multiple self (pp. 79–92). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • De Houwer, J. , Teige-Mocigemba, S. , Spruyt, A. , Moors, A. (2009). Implicit measures: A normative analysis and review. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 347–368. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dessau, B. , Kanitscheider, B. (2000). Von Lust und Freude. Frankfurt a.M.: Insel. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Deutsch, R. , Strack, F. (2006). Duality Models in Social Psychology: Response to Commentaries. Psychological Inquiry, 17, 265–268. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dislich, F. X. R. , Zinkernagel, A. , Ortner, T. M. , Schmitt, M. (2010). Convergence of direct, indirect, and objective risk taking measures in the domain of gambling: The moderating role of impulsiveness and self-control. Journal of Psychology, 218, 20–27. First citation in articleAbstractGoogle Scholar

  • Dwyer, D. M. , Jarratt, F. , Dick, K. (2007). Evaluative conditioning with foods as CSs and body shapes as USs: No evidence for sex differences, extinction, or overshadowing. Cognition & Emotion, 21, 281–299. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Eder, A. B. , Rothermund, K. (2008). When do motor behaviors (mis)match affective stimuli? An evaluative coding view of approach and avoidance reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137, 262–281. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Epstein, S. (1990). Cognitive-experiential self-theory. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality. Theory and research (pp. 165–192). New York: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Erk, S. , Kleczar, A. , Walter, H. (2007). Valence-specific regulation effects in a working memory task with emotional context. Neuroimage, 37, 623–632. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Evans, J. S. B. T. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 255–278. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Eysenck, S. B. G. , Daum, I. , Schugens, M. M. , Diehl, J. M. (1990). A cross-cultural study of impulsiveness, venturesomeness and empathy: Germany and England. Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie, 11, 209–213. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Fedoroff, I. C. , Polivy, J. , Herman, C. P. (1997). The effect of pre-exposure to food cues on the eating behavior of restrained and unrestrained eaters. Appetite, 28, 33–47. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fiedler, K. , Messner, C. , Bluemke, M. (2006). Unresolved problems with the „I”, the „A”, and the „T”: A logical and psychometric critique of the Implicit Association Test (IAT). European Review of Social Psychology, 17, 74–147. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fishbach, A. , Friedman, R. S. , Kruglanski, A. W. (2003). Leading us not unto temptation: Momentary allurements elicit overriding goal activation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 296–309. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fishbein, M. , Ajzen, I. (Eds.). (1975). Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: An introduction to theory and research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Florack, A. , Friese, M. , Scarabis, M. (2010). Regulatory focus and reliance on implicit processes in consumption contexts. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 20, 193–204. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fodor, J. A. (1983). Modularity of Mind: An Essay on Faculty Psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Frank, M. J. , Cohen, M. X. , Sanfey, A. G. (2009). Multiple systems in decision making. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 73–77. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Freud, S. (1923). Das Ich und das Es. In S. Freud (Hrsg.), Das Ich und das Es und andere metapsychologische Schriften (1960) (S. 171–208). Frankfurt a.M.: Fischer Taschenbuch. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Freud, S. (1934). Neue Folge der Vorlesungen zur Einführung in die Psychoanalyse. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Friese, M. , Bargas-Avila, J. , Hofmann, W. , Wiers, R. W. (2010). Here’s looking at you, Bud: Memory associations with alcohol predict attentional bias for social drinkers with low executive control. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 1, 143–151. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Friese, M. , Hofmann, W. (2008). What would you have as a last supper? Thoughts about death influence evaluation and consumption of food products. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 1388–1394. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Friese, M. , Hofmann, W. (2009). Control me or I will control you: Impulse, trait self-control, and the guidance of behavior. Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 795–805. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Friese, M. , Hofmann, W. , Wänke, M. (2008). When impulses take over: Moderated predictive validity of implicit and explicit attitude measures in predicting food choice and consumption behaviour. British Journal of Social Psychology, 47, 397–419. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gailliot, M. T. , Baumeister, R. F. (2006). Self-regulation and sexual restraint: Dispositionally and temporarily poor self-regulatory abilities contribute to failures at restraining sexual behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 173–186. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gawronski, B. , Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006). Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: An integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 692–731. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gawronski, B. , Conrey, F. R. (2004). Der Implizite Assoziationstest als Maß automatisch aktivierter Assoziationen: Reichweite und Grenzen. Psychologische Rundschau, 55, 118–126. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Gibbons, F. X. , Gerrard, M. , Blanton, H. , Russell, D. W. (1998). Reasoned action and social reaction: Willingness and intention as independent predictors of health risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1164–1181. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54, 493. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Goschke, T. (2004). Vom freien Willen zur Selbstdetermination: Kognitive und volititionale Mechanismen der intentionalen Handlungssteuerung. Psychologische Rundschau, 55, 186–197. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Granö, N. , Virtanen, M. , Vaherta, J. , Elovainio, M. , Kivimäki, M. (2004). Impulsivity as a predictor of smoking and alcohol consumption. Personality and Individual Differences, 37, 1693–1700. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gray, J. A. (1987). The neuropsychology of anxiety. Oxford: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A. G. , McGhee, D. E. , Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Grenard, J. L. , Ames, S. L. , Wiers, R. W. , Thush, C. , Sussman, S. , Stacy, A. W. (2008). Working memory capacity moderates the predictive effects of drug-related associations on substance use. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 22, 426–432. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Guckes, B. (2005). Willensschwäche und Zwang. Zeitschrift für philosophische Forschung, 59, 179–201. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Herman, C. P. , Polivy, J. , Lank, C. , Heatherton, T. F. (1987). Anxiety, hunger and eating behavior. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96, 264–269. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hill, T. E. (2005). Willensschwäche und Charakter. In T. Spitzley (Hrsg.), Willensschwäche (S. 168–190). Paderborn: mentis. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hoch, S. J. , Loewenstein, G. F. (1991). Time-inconsistent preferences and consumer self-control. Journal of Consumer Research, 17, 492. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hoefling, A. , Strack, F. (2008). The tempting effect of forbidden foods. High calorie content evokes conflicting implicit and explicit evaluations in restrained eaters. Appetite, 51, 681–689. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hofmann, W. , Deutsch, R. , Lancaster, K. , Banaji, M. R. (2010). Cooling the heat of temptation: Mental self-control and the automatic evaluation of tempting stimuli. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 17–25. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hofmann, W. , Friese, M. , Roefs, A. (2009). Three ways to resist temptation: The independent contributions of executive attention, inhibitory control, and affect regulation to the impulse control of eating behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 431–435. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hofmann, W. , Friese, M. , Strack, F. (2009). Impulse and self-control from a dual-systems perspective. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 162–176. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hofmann, W. , Gawronski, B. , Gschwendner, T. , Le, H. , Schmitt, M. (2005). A meta-analysis on the correlation between the Implicit Association Test and explicit self-report measures. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 1369–1385. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hofmann, W. , Gschwendner, T. , Castelli, L. , Schmitt, M. (2008). Implicit and explicit attitudes and interracial interaction: The moderating role of situationally available control resources. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 11, 69–87. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hofmann, W. , Gschwendner, T. , Friese, M. , Wiers, R. W. , Schmitt, M. (2008). Working memory capacity and self-regulatory behavior: Toward an individual differences perspective on behavior determination by automatic versus controlled processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 962–977. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hofmann, W. , Rauch, W. , Gawronski, B. (2007). And deplete us not into temptation: Automatic attitudes, dietary re-straint, and self-regulatory resources as determinants of eating behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 497–504. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hofmann, W. , Friese, M. , Schmeichel, B. J. , Baddeley, A. D. (2010). Working memory and self-regulation. In K. D. Vohs, R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), The handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (pp. 204–225). New York: Guilford Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hofmann, W. , Schmitt, M. (2008). Editorial: Advances and Challenges in the Indirect Measurement of Individual Differences at Age 10 of the Implicit Association Test. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 24, 207–209. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Hofmann, W. , Wilson, T. D. (2010). Consciousness, introspection, and the adaptive unconscious. In B. Gawronski, B. K. Payne (Eds.), Handbook of implicit social cognition: Measurement, Therapie and Applications (pp. 197–215). New York: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Holland, R. W. , Hermsen, B. , van Knippenberg, A. (2008). The happy act on impulse, the sad think twice: Mood as a moderator of the impact of implicit and explicit attitudes on behavior. Unpublished manuscript. Radboud Universiteit Nijmegen. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Holton, R. (1999). Intention and weakness of will. The Journal of Philosophy, 96, 241–262. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Holton, R. (2003). How is Strength of Will Possible? In S. Stroud, C. Tappolet (Eds.), Weakness of Will and Practical Irrationality (pp. 39–67). Oxford: Clarendon Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hull, J. G. , Bond, C. F. (1986). Social and behavioral consequences of alcohol consumption and expectancy: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 347–360. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • James, W. (1950). The principles of psychology (Vol. 1). New York: Dover (Original work published 1890). First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Jansen, A. , van den Hout, M. (1991). On being led into temptation: „Counterregulation” of dieters after smelling a „preload”. Addictive Behaviors, 16, 247–253. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Janz, N. K. , Becker, M. H. (1984). The health belief model: A decade later. Health Education Quarterly, 11, 1–47. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kavanagh, D. J. , Andrade, J. , May, J. (2005). Imaginary relish and exquisite torture: The Elaborated Intrusion theory of desire. Psychological Review, 112, 446–467. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kenrick, D. T. , Funder, D. C. (1988). Profiting from controversy: Lessons from the person-situation debate. American Psychologist, 43, 23–34. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Keren, G. , Schul, Y. (2009). Two is not always better than one: A critical evaluation of two-system theories. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 533–549. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Klauer, K. C. , Voss, A. , Schmitz, F. , Teige-Mocigemba, S. (2007). Process components of the Implicit Association Test: A diffusion-model analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 353–368. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Krieglmeyer, R. , Deutsch, R. , De Houwer, J. , De Raedt, R. (2010). Being moved: Valence activates approach-avoidance behavior independent of evaluation and approach-avoidance intentions. Psychological Science, 21, 607–613. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kruglanski, A. W. , Erb, H. P. , Pierro, A. , Mannetti, L. , Chun, W. Y. (2006). On parametric continuities in the world of binary either ors. Psychological Inquiry, 17, 153–165. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kruglanski, A. W. , Shah, J. Y. , Fishbach, A. , Friedman, R. , Chun, W. Y. , Sleeth-Keppler, D. (2002). A theory of goal systems. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 34, pp. 331–378). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kuhl, J. (2000). A functional-design approach to motivation and volition: The dynamics of personality systems interactions. In M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, M. Zeidner (Eds.), Self-regulation: Directions and challenges for future research (pp. 111–169). New York: Academic Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kuhl, J. (2001). Motivation und Persönlichkeit: Interaktionen psychischer Systeme. Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kuhl, J. , Goschke, T. (1994). A theory of action control: Mental subsystems, modes of control, and volitional conflictresolution strategies. In J. Kuhl, J. Beckmann (Eds.), Volition and personality: Action versus state orientation (pp. 93–124). Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kunda, Z. (1990). The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 480–498. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lascelles, K. R. R. , Field, A. P. , Davey, G. C. L. (2003). Using foods as CSs and body shapes as UCSs: Selective conditioning effects during picturepicture evaluative conditioning. Behavior Therapy, 34, 213–235. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lieberman, M. D. (2007). The X- and C-systems: The neural basis of automatic and controlled social cognition. In E. Harmon-Jones, P. Winkelman (Eds.), Fundamentals of Social Neuroscience (pp. 290–315). New York: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Livnat, A. , Pippenger, N. (2006). An optimal brain can be composed of conflicting agents. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 103, 3198–3202. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Loewenstein, G. , Elster, J. (1992). Choice over tim. New York: Sage. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Magnusson, D. , Endler, N. S. (1977). Interactional psychology: Present status and future prospects. In D. Magnusson, N. S. Endler (Eds.), Personality at the crossroads (pp. 3–31). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Metcalfe, J. , Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool system analysis of delay of gratification: Dynamics of willpower. Psychological Review, 106, 3–19. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mierke, J. , Klauer, K. C. (2003). Method-specific variance in the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 1180–1192. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Miller, E. K. , Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 167–202. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Miller, G. A. , Galanter, E. , Pribram, K. H. (1960). Plans and the Structure of Behavior. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mischel, W. (1974). Processes in delay of gratification. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 7, pp. 249–292). San Diego: Academic. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Mischel, W. , Baker, N. (1975). Cognitive appraisals and transformations in delay behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 254–261. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mischel, W. , Shoda, Y. , Peake, P. K. (1988). The nature of adolescent competencies predicted by preschool delay of gratification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 687–696. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Miyake, A. , Friedman, N. P. , Emerson, M. J. , Witzki, A. H. , Howerter, A. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex „frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Morsella, E. (2005). The function of phenomenal states: Supramodular interaction theory. Psychological Review, 112, 1000–1021. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Müller, J. (2007). Zerrissener Wille, Willensschwäche und menschliche Freiheit bei Augustinus: Eine analytisch motivierte Kontextualisierung von Confessiones VIII. Philosophisches Jahrbuch, 114, 49–72. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Müller, J. (2009). Willensschwäche in Antike und Mittelalter. Eine Problemgeschichte von Sokrates bis Johannes Duns Scotus. Leuven: Leuven University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Muraven, M. , Collins, R. L. , Nienhaus, K. (2002). Self-control and alcohol restraint: An initial application of the self-control strength model. Psychology of Addictive Behavior, 16, 113–120. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nederkoorn, C. , Houben, K. , Hofmann, W. , Roefs, A. , Jansen, A. (2010). Control yourself or just eat what you like? Weight gain over a year is predicted by an interactive effect of response inhibition and a preference for high fat foods. Health Psychology, 29, 389–393. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Neumann, R. , Hülsenbeck, K. , Seibt, B. (2004). Attitudes toward people with AIDS and avoidance behavior: Automatic and reflective bases of behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 543–550. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nisbett, R. E. , Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling More Than We Can Know: Verbal Reports on Mental Processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Nordgren, L. F. , van der Pligt, J. , van Harreveld, F. (2007). Evaluating Eve: visceral states influence the evaluation of impulsive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 75–84. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Norman, D. A. , Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action. Willed and automatic control of behavior. In R. J. Davidson, G. E. Schwartz, D. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and self regulation: Advances in research (pp. 1–18). New York: Plenum Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ostafin, B. D. , Marlatt, G. A. , Greenwald, A. G. (2008). Drinking without thinking: An implicit measure of alcohol motivation predicts failure to control alcohol use. Behavior Research and Therapy, 46, 1210–1219. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Papies, E. , Stroebe, W. , Aarts, H. (2007). Pleasure in the mind: restrained eating and spontaneous hedonic thoughts about food. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 810–817. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Payne, B. K. , Cheng, C. M. , Govorun, O. , Stewart, B. D. (2005). An inkblot for attitudes: Affect misattribution as implicit measurement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 277–293. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Payne, B. K. , Govorun, O. , Arbuckle, N. L. (2008). Automatic attitudes and alcohol: Does implicit liking predict drinking? Cognition and Emotion, 22, 238–271. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Phelps, E. A. , O’Connor, K. J. , Cunningham, W. A. , Funayama, E. S. , Gatenby, J. C. , Gore, J. C. , et al. (2000). Performance on indirect measures of race evaluation predicts amygdala activation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 729–738. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ridderinkhof, R. , Forstmann, B. , Wylie, S. , Burle, B. , van den Wildenberg, W. (2010). Neurocognitive mechanisms of action control: Resisting the call of the sirens. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 2, 174–192. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rinck, M. , Becker, E. S. (2007). Approach and avoidance in fear of spiders. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 38, 105–120. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rogers, R. W. (1983). Cognitive and physiological processes in fear appraisal and attitude change: A revised theory of protection motivation. In J. Cacioppo, R. Petty (Eds.), Social Psychophysiology (pp. 153–176). New York: Guilford Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Rosenstock, I. M. (1966). Why people use health services. Milbank Memorial Fund Quarterly, 44, 94–124. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rothbart, M. K. , Ellis, L. K. , Rueda, M. R. , Posner, M. I. (2003). Developing mechanisms of temperamental effortful control. Journal of Personality, 71, 1113–1143. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Roughley, N. (2008). Willensschwäche und Personsein. In H. Tegtmeyer, F. Kannetzky (Hrsg.), Personalität. Studien zu einem Schlüsselbegriff der Philosophie (S. 143–161). Leipzig: Universitätsverlag. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Saunders, D. R. (1956). Moderator variables in prediction. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 16, 209–222. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sayette, M. A. , Loewenstein, G. , Griffin, K. M. , Black, J. J. (2008). Exploring the cold-to-hot empathy gap in smokers. Psychological Science, 19, 926–932. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Scarabis, M. , Florack, A. , Gosejohann, S. (2006). When consumers follow their feelings: The impact of affective or cognitive focus on the basis of consumers’ choice. Psychology & Marketing, 23, 1015–1034. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schmitt, M. (1990). Konsistenz als Persönlichkeitseigenschaft? Moderatorvariablen in der Persönlichkeits- und Einstellungsforschung. Berlin: Springer. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schoenmakers, T. , Wiers, R. W. , Jones, B. T. , Bruce, G. , Jansen, A. T. M. (2007). Attentional retraining decreases attentional bias in heavy drinkers without generalization. Addiction, 102, 399–405. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schwarzer, R. (1999). Self-regulatory processes in the adoption and maintenance of health behaviors: The role of optimism, goals, and threats. Journal of Health Psychology, 4, 115–127. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Seibt, B. , Häfner, M. , Deutsch, R. (2007). Prepared to eat: How immediate affective and motivational responses to food cues are influenced by food deprivation. European Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 359–379. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Shah, J. Y. , Friedman, R. S. , Kruglanski, A. W. (2002). Forgetting all else: On the antecedents and consequences of goal shielding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1261–1280. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sherman, J. W. , Gawronski, B. , Gonsalkorale, K. , Hugenberg, K. , Allen, T. J. , Groom, C. J. (2008). The self-regulation of automatic associations and behavioral impulses. Psychological Review, 115, 314–335. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Six, B. , Eckes, T. (1996). Metaanalysen in der Einstellungs-Verhaltens-Forschung. Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie, 27, 7–17. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Sloman, S. A. (1996). The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 3–22. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Smith, E. C. , DeCoster, J. (2000). Dual-process models in social and cognitive psychology: Conceptual integration and links to underlying memory systems. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4, 108–131. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Strack, F. , Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 220–247. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stroebe, W. (2002). Übergewicht als Schicksal? Die kognitive Steuerung des Essverhaltens. [Overweight as fate? On the cognitive regulation of eating behavior]. Psychologische Rundschau, 53, 14–22. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Stroebe, W. , Mensink, W. , Aarts, H. , Schut, H. , Kruglanski, A. (2008). Why dieters fail: Testing the goal conflict model of eating. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 26–36. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tangney, J. P. , Baumeister, R. F. , Boone, A. L. (2004). High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. Journal of Personality, 72, 271–324. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Thush, C. , Wiers, R. W. , Ames, S. L. , Grenard, J. L. , Sussman, S. , Stacy, A. W. (2008). Interactions between implicit and explicit cognition and working memory capacity in the prediction of alcohol use in at-risk adolescents. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 94, 116–124. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Towler, G. , Shepherd, R. (1991). Modification of Fishbein and Ajzen’s theory of reasoned action to predict chip consumption. Food Quality and Preference, 3, 37–45. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Verbruggen, F. , Logan, G. D. (2008). Automatic and controlled response inhibition: Associative learning in the go/no-go and stop-signal paradigms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137, 649–672. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Verdejo-Garcia, A. , Lawrence, A. J. , Clark, L. (2008). Impulsivity as a vulnerability marker for substance-use disorders: Review of findings from high-risk research, problem gamblers and genetic association studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 777–810. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Verplanken, B. , Orbell, S. (2003). Reflections on past behavior: A self-report index of habit strength. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 33, 1313–1330. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vohs, K. D. , Heatherton, T. F. (2000). Self-regulatory failure: A resource-depletion approach. Psychological Science, 11, 249–254. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Waldeck, T. L. , Miller, L. S. (1997). Gender and impulsivity differences in licit substance use. Journal of Substance Abuse, 9, 269–275. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ward, A. , Mann, T. (2000). Don’t mind if I do: Disinhibited eating under cognitive load. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 753–763. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Watson, G. (2004). Disordered appetites: Addiction, compulsion, and dependency. In G. Watson (Ed.), Agency and Answerability (pp. 59–87). Oxford: Clarendon Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wiers, R. W. , Beckers, L. , Houben, K. , Hofmann, W. (2009). A short fuse after alcohol: Implicit power associations predict aggressiveness after alcohol consumption in young heavy drinkers with limited executive control. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 93, 300–305. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wiers, R. W. , Dictus, M. , Houben, K. , Van den Wildenberg, E. , Rinck, M. (2008). Strong automatic appetitive action tendencies in heavy drinkers with a g-allele in a Functional Polymorphism of the Mu-Opioid Receptor Gene (OPRM1). Manuscript submitted for publication. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Wiers, R. W. , Rinck, M. , Kordts, R. , Houben, K. , Strack, F. (2008). Re-training automatic action-tendencies to approach alcohol in hazardous drinkers. Manuscript submitted for publication. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Wilson, T. D. , Lindsey, S. , Schooler, T. Y. (2000). A model of dual attitudes. Psychological Review, 107, 101–126. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wühr, P. , Kunde, W. (2008). Die kognitive Regulation von Handlungskonflikten. Psychologische Rundschau, 59, 207–216. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Zuckerman, M. (1985). Biological foundations of the sensation-seeking temperament. In J. Strelau, F. H. Farley, A. Gale (Eds.), The biological bases of personality and behavior: Vol. 1. Theories, measurement techniques, and development (pp. 97–113). Washington, DC: Hemisphere. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar