Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803.22.2.91

Neuroimaging studies have shown that the face-related regions of the brain, including the amygdala and the fusiform gyrus, are activated when subjects viewed the faces of their own race as compared to those of other races. These regions have been regarded as the neural correlates of “in-group advantage,” a phenomenon showing superior performance for the same-race face than for other-race faces in behavioral experiments. However, in these neuroimaging studies, faces of notably different races have been used for comparisons, and thus far, no study has investigated brain activity associated with positive expression. In the present functional magnetic resonance imaging study involving the Japanese participants, we investigated brain activation in response to viewing happy and neutral faces of Japanese, non-Japanese Asian, and Caucasian races. Analysis of covariance using the mean happiness rating as a covariate revealed that as compared to viewing of neutral faces, viewing of happy faces by the Japanese subjects resulted in greater activation of the posterior cingulate cortex under the same-race condition than under other-race conditions. In addition, parametric modulation analysis showed that clusters in a part of the posterior cingulate cortex and in the superior temporal gyrus were associated with subjective judgment of the facial features that resembled those of the Japanese race. The left amygdala activity differed between the races; however, it is more likely that this difference was related to the differences in the subjective rating of facial happiness than to the recognition of the race per se. Therefore, we considered that the posterior cingulate cortex is the neural correlate that is specifically involved when Japanese subjects judge the happy expression on the same-race face. These results may indicate that in the Japanese participants a sense of familiarity played a role in the neural processing of a positive expression on the same-race face.

References

  • Adolphs, R. , Tranel, D. , Hamann, S. , Young, A.W. , Calder, A.J. , Phelps, E.A. et al. (1999). Recognition of facial emotion in nine individuals with bilateral amygdala damage. Neuropsychologia, 37, 1111–1117. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brett, M. , Anton, J. , Valabregue, R. , Polini, J.P. (2002). Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox. In 8th International Conference on Functional Mapping of the Human Brain. Sendai, Japan: Elsevier. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Cunningham, W.A. , Johnson, M.K. , Raye, C.L. , Chris Gatenby, J. , Gore, J.C. , Banaji, M.R. (2004). Separable neural components in the processing of black and white faces. Psychological Science, 15, 806–813. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Elfenbein, H.A. , Ambady, N. (2002). On the universality and cultural specificity of emotion recognition: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 203–235. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fitzgerald, D.A. , Angstadt, M. , Jelsone, L.M. , Nathan, P.J. , Phan, K.L. (2006). Beyond threat: Amygdala reactivity across multiple expressions of facial affect. Neuroimage, 30, 1441–1448. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gobbini, M.I. , Haxby, J.V. (2007). Neural systems for recognition of familiar faces. Neuropsychologia, 45, 32–41. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gobbini, M.I. , Leibenluft, E. , Santiago, N. , Haxby, J.V. (2004). Social and emotional attachment in the neural representation of faces. Neuroimage, 22, 1628–35. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Golby, A.J. , Gabrieli, J.D. , Chiao, J.Y. , Eberhardt, J.L. (2001). Differential responses in the fusiform region to same-race and other-race faces. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 845–850. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gorno-Tempini, M.L. , Price, C.J. , Josephs, O. , Vandenberghe, R. , Cappa, S.F. , Kapur, N. , Frackowiak, R.S. (1998). The neural systems sustaining face and proper-name processing. Brain, 121, 2103–2118. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Grimshaw, G.M. , Bulman-Fleming, M.B. , Ngo, C. (2004). A signal-detection analysis of sex differences in the perception of emotional faces. Brain and Cognition, 54, 248–250. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hart, A.J. , Whalen, P.J. , Shin, L.M. , Mcinerney, S.C. , Fischer, H. , Rauch, S.L. (2000). Differential response in the human amygdala to racial outgroup vs ingroup face stimuli. Neuroreport, 11, 2351–2355. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Haxby, J.V. , Hoffman, E.A. , Gobbini, M.I. (2002). Human neural systems for face recognition and social communication. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 59–67. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hennenlotter, A. , Schroeder, U. , Erhard, P. , Castrop, F. , Haslinger, B. , Stoecker, D. et al. (2005). A common neural basis for receptive and expressive communication of pleasant facial affect. Neuroimage, 26, 581–591. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Horstmann, G. , Bauland, A. (2006). Search asymmetries with real faces: Testing the anger-superiority effect. Emotion, 6, 193–207. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hugdahl, K. , Iversen, P.M. , Johnsen, B.H. (1993). Laterality for facial expressions: Does the sex of the subject interact with the sex of the stimulus face? Cortex, 29, 325–331. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kim, J.S. , Yoon, H.W. , Kim, B.S. , Jeun, S.S. , Jung, S.L. , Choe, B.Y. (2006). Racial distinction of the unknown facial identity recognition mechanism by event-related fMRI. Neuroscience Letters, 397, 279–284. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Leibenluft, E. , Gobbini, M.I. , Harrison, T. , Haxby, J.V. (2004). Mothers’ neural activation in response to pictures of their children and other children. Biological Psychiatry, 56, 225–232. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Leppanen, J.M. , Hietanen, J.K. (2004). Positive facial expressions are recognized faster than negative facial expressions, but why? Psychological Research, 69, 22–29. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lieberman, M.D. , Hariri, A. , Jarcho, J.M. , Eisenberger, N.I. , Bookheimer, S.Y. (2005). An fMRI investigation of race-related amygdala activity in African-American and Caucasian-American individuals. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 720–722. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Moriguchi, Y. , Ohnishi, T. , Kawachi, T. , Mori, T. , Hirakata, M. , Yamada, M. et al. (2005). Specific brain activation in Japanese and Caucasian people to fearful faces. Neuroreport, 16, 133–136. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Phelps, E.A. , O’Connor, K.J. , Cunningham, W.A. , Funayama, E.S. , Gatenby, J.C. , Gore, J.C. et al. (2000). Performance on indirect measures of race evaluation predicts amygdala activation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 729–738. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Puce, A. , Allison, T. , Bentin, S. , Gore, J.C. , McCarthy, G. (1998). Temporal cortex activation in humans viewing eye and mouth movements. Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 2188–2199. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Shah, N.J. , Marshall, J.C. , Zafiris, O. , Schwab, A. , Zilles, K. , Markowitsch, H.J. et al. (2001). The neural correlates of person familiarity. A functional magnetic resonance imaging study with clinical implications. Brain, 124, 804–815. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Shimamura, A.P. , Ross, J.G. , Bennett, H.D. (2006). Memory for facial expressions: The power of a smile. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13, 217–222. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wagner, A.D. , Shannon, B.J. , Kahn, I. , Buckner, R.L. (2005). Parietal lobe contributions to episodic memory retrieval. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 445–453. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Williams, M.A. , Mcglone, F. , Abbott, D.F. , Mattingley, J.B. (2005). Differential amygdala responses to happy and fearful facial expressions depend on selective attention. Neuroimage, 24, 417–425. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zald, D.H. (2003). The human amygdala and the emotional evaluation of sensory stimuli. Brain Research Reviews, 41, 88–123. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar