Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000078

We investigated whether amygdala activation, autonomic responses, respiratory responses, and facial muscle activity (measured over the brow and cheek [fear grin] regions) are all sensitive to phobic versus nonphobic fear and, more importantly, whether effects in these variables vary as a function of both phobic and nonphobic fear intensity. Spider-phobic and comparably low spider-fearful control participants imagined encountering different animals and rated their subjective fear while their central and peripheral nervous system activity was measured. All measures included in our study were sensitive to variations in subjective fear, but were related to different ranges and positions on the subjective fear level continuum. Left amygdala activation, heart rate, and facial muscle activity over the cheek region captured fear intensity variations even within narrowly described regions on the fear level continuum (here within extremely low levels of fear and within considerable phobic fear). Skin conductance and facial muscle activity over the brow region did not capture fear intensity variations within low levels of fear: skin conductance mirrored only extreme levels of fear, and activity over the brow region distinguished phobic from nonphobic fear but also low-to-moderate and high phobic fear. Finally, respiratory measures distinguished phobic from nonphobic fear with no further differentiation within phobic and nonphobic fear. We conclude that a careful consideration of the measures to be used in an investigation and the population to be examined can be critical in order to obtain significant results.

References

  • Alpers, G. W. , Wilhelm, F. H. , Roth, W. T. (2005). Psychophysiological assessment during exposure in driving phobic patients. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 126–139. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • American Psychiatric Association . (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Armony, J. L. , Dolan, R. J. (2002). Modulation of spatial attention by fear-conditioned stimuli: An event-related fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 40, 817–826. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Aue, T. , Flykt, A. , Scherer, K. R. (2007). First evidence for differential and sequential efferent effects of goal relevance and goal conduciveness appraisal. Biological Psychology, 74, 347–357. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Aue, T. , Hoeppli, M. E. (2012). Evidence for an encounter expectancy bias in fear of spiders. Cognition and Emotion, 26, 727–736. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Aue, T. , Scherer, K. R. (2008). Appraisal-driven somatovisceral response patterning: Effects of intrinsic pleasantness and goal conduciveness. Biological Psychology, 79, 158–164. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Aue, T. , Scherer, K. R. (2011). Effects of intrinsic pleasantness and goal conduciveness appraisals on somatovisceral responding: Somewhat similar, but not identical. Biological Psychology, 86, 65–73. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bradley, M. M. , Lang, P. (2007). Emotion and motivation. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, G. G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology (3rd ed., pp. 581–607). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carlsson, K. , Petersson, K. M. , Lundqvist, D. , Karlsson, A. , Ingvar, M. , Öhman, A. (2004). Fear and the amygdala: Manipulation of awareness generates differential cerebral responses to phobic and fear-relevant (but nonfeared) stimuli. Emotion, 4, 340–353. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dan-Glauser, E. S. (2008). Vivre son émotion: Mécanismes d’induction et particularités physiologiques d’un processus émotionnel débouchant sur un sentiment subjectif [Living emotions: Induction mechanisms and physiological characteristics of an emotional process leading to a subjective feeling state]. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Dan-Glauser, E. S. , Scherer, K. R. (2011). The Geneva Affective PicturE Database (GAPED): A new 730 picture database focusing on valence and normative significance. Behavior Research Methods, 43, 468–477. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Davis, M. , Lang, P. J. (2003). Emotion. In M. Gallagher, R. J. Nelson (Eds.), Handbook of psychology: Biological psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 405–439). New York, NY: Wiley. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dimberg, U. , Hansson, G. , Thunberg, M. (1998). Fear of snakes and facial reactions: A case of rapid responding. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 39, 75–80. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fredrikson, M. , Sundin, Ö. , Frankenhaeuser, M. (1985). Cortisol excretion during the defense reaction in humans. Psychosomatic Medicine, 47, 313–319. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fridlund, A. J. , Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Guidelines for human electromyographic research. Psychophysiology, 23, 567–589. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Friston, K. J. , Holmes, A. P. , Worsley, K. J. , Poline, J. B. , Frith, C. D. , Frackowiak, R. S. J. (1995). Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach. Human Brain Mapping, 2, 189–210. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gemignani, A. , Santarcangelo, E. , Sebastiani, L. , Marchese, C. , Mammoliti, R. , Simoni, A. , Ghelarducci, B. (2000). Changes in autonomic and EEG patterns induced by hypnotic imagination of aversive stimuli in man. Brain Research Bulletin, 53, 105–111. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gerlach, A. L. , Spellmeyer, G. , Vögele, C. , Huster, R. , Stevens, S. , Hetzel, G. , Deckert, J. (2006). Blood-injury phobia with and without a history of fainting: Disgust sensitivity does not explain the fainting response. Psychosomatic Medicine, 68, 331–339. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Globisch, J. , Hamm, A. O. , Esteves, F. , Öhman, A. (1999). Fear appears fast: Temporal course of startle reflex potentiation in animal fearful subjects. Psychophysiology, 36, 66–75. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hamm, A. O. , Cuthbert, B. N. , Globisch, J. , Vaitl, D. (1997). Fear and the startle reflex: Blink modulation and autonomic response patterns in animal and mutilation fearful subjects. Psychophysiology, 34, 97–107. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hamm, A. , Weike, A. I. (2005). The neuropsychology of fear learning and fear regulation. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 57, 5–14. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Harrigan, J. A. , O’Connell, D. M. (1996). How do you look when feeling anxious? Facial displays of anxiety. Personality and Individual Differences, 21, 205–212. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Knopf, K. , Pössel, P. (2009). Individual-response differences in spider phobia: Comparing phobic and nonphobic women of different reactivity levels. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 22, 39–55. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lang, P. J. , Bradley, M. M. , Cuthbert, B. N. (1998). Emotion, motivation and anxiety: Brain mechanisms and psychophysiology. Biological Psychiatry, 44, 1248–1263. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • LeDoux, J. E. (1996). The emotional brain. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • LeDoux, J. E. , Phelps, E. A. (2000). Emotional networks in the brain. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 157–172). New York, NY: Guilford Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Morris, J. S. , Öhman, A. , Dolan, R. J. (1998). Conscious and unconscious emotional learning in the human amygdala. Nature, 393, 467–470. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Morris, J. S. , Öhman, A. , Dolan, R. J. (1999). A subcortical pathway to the right amygdala mediating “unseen” fear. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 96, 1680–1685. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mühlberger, A. , Wiedemann, G. , Herrmann, M. J. , Pauli, P. (2006). Phylo- and ontogenetic fears and the expectation of danger: Differences between spider- and flight-phobic subjects in cognitive and physiological responses to disorder-specific stimuli. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115, 580–589. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Roth, W. T. (2005). Physiological markers for anxiety: Panic disorder and phobias. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 58, 190–198. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sarlo, M. , Palomba, D. , Angrilli, A. , Stegagno, L. (2002). Blood phobia and spider phobia: Two specific phobias with different autonomic cardiac modulations. Biological Psychology, 60, 91–108. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. , Landeau, B. , Papathanassiou, D. , Crivello, F. , Etard, O. , Delcroix, N. , … Joliot, M. (2002). Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. NeuroImage, 15, 273–289. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Van Hooff, J. A. (1972). A comparative approach to the phylogeny of laughter and smiling. In R. Hinde (Ed.), Nonverbal communication (pp. 209–241). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Weike, A. I. , Hamm, A. , Schupp, H. T. , Runge, U. , Schröder, H. W. S. , Kessler, C. (2005). Fear conditioning following unilateral temporal lobectomy: Dissociation of conditioned startle potentiation and autonomic learning. The Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 11117–11124. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wendt, J. , Lotze, M. , Weike, A. I. , Hosten, N. , Hamm, A. O. (2008). Brain activation and defensive response mobilization during sustained exposure to phobia-related and other affective pictures in spider phobia. Psychophysiology, 45, 205–215. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wilhelm, F. H. , Roth, W. T. (1998). Taking the laboratory to the skies: Ambulatory assessment of self-report, autonomic, and respiratory responses in flying phobia. Psychophysiology, 35, 596–606. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • World Health Organization . (1992). International classification of diseases and related health problems (10th ed.). Geneva, Switzerland: Author. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar