Skip to main content
Article

The Relationship Between Interoception and Metacognition

A Pilot Study

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000157

Abstract. Recently, Garfinkel and Critchley (2013) proposed to distinguish between three facets of interoception: interoceptive sensibility, interoceptive accuracy, and interoceptive awareness. This pilot study investigated how these facets interrelate to each other and whether interoceptive awareness is related to the metacognitive awareness of memory performance. A sample of 24 healthy students completed a heartbeat perception task (HPT) and a memory task. Judgments of confidence were requested for each task. Participants filled in questionnaires assessing interoceptive sensibility, depression, anxiety, and socio-demographic characteristics. The three facets of interoception were found to be uncorrelated and interoceptive awareness was not related to metacognitive awareness of memory performance. Whereas memory performance was significantly related to metamemory awareness, interoceptive accuracy (HPT) and interoceptive awareness were not correlated. Results suggest that future research on interoception should assess all facets of interoception in order to capture the multifaceted quality of the construct.

References

  • Alba, J. W. & Hutchinson, J. W. (2000). Knowledge calibration: What consumers know and what they think they know. Journal of Consumer Research, 27, 123–156. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Arbel, R., Koren, D., Klein, E. & Latzer, Y. (2013). The neurocognitive basis of insight into illness in anorexia nervosa: A pilot metacognitive study. Psychiatry Research, 209, 604–610. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baayen, R. H., Piepenbrock, R. & Van Rijn, H. (1993). The CELEX lexical database on CD-ROM. Philadelphia, PA: Linguistic Data Consortium. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Buratti, S. & Allwood, C. M. (2012). The accuracy of meta-metacognitive judgments: Regulating the realism of confidence. Cognitive Processing, 13, 243–253. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ceunen, E., Van Diest, I. & Vlaeyen, J. W. S. (2013). Accuracy and awareness of perception: Related, yet distinct (commentary on Herbert et al., 2012). Biological Psychology, 92, 426–427. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Craig, A. D. (2003). Interoception: The sense of the physiological condition of the body. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13, 500–505. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Critchley, H. D., Wiens, S., Rotshtein, P., Ohman, A. & Dolan, R. J. (2004). Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 189–195. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • DiMatteo, M. R., Lepper, H. S. & Croghan, T. W. (2000). Depression is a risk factor for noncompliance with medical treatment – Meta-analysis of the effects of anxiety and depression on patient adherence. Archives of Internal Medicine, 160, 2101–2107. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dunlosky, J. & Matvey, G. (2001). Empirical analysis of the intrinsic-extrinsic distinction of judgments of learning (JOLs): Effects of relatedness and serial position on JOLs. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition, 27, 1180–1191. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dunn, B. D., Dalgleish, T., Lawrence, A. D. & Ogilvie, A. D. (2007). The accuracy of self-monitoring and its relationship to self-focused attention in dysphoria and clinical depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 1–15. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dunn, B. D., Dalgleish, T., Ogilvie, A. D. & Lawrence, A. D. (2007). Heartbeat perception in depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 1921–1930. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dunn, B. D., Stefanovitch, I., Evans, D., Oliver, C., Hawkins, A. & Dalgleish, T. (2010). Can you feel the beat? Interoceptive awareness is an interactive function of anxiety- and depression-specific symptom dimensions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48, 1133–1138. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dunning, D., Johnson, K., Ehrlinger, J. & Kruger, J. (2003). Why people fail to recognize their own incompetence. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 83–87. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ehlers, A. & Breuer, P. (1992). Increased cardiac awareness in panic disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 371–382. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ehlers, A. & Breuer, P. (1996). How good are patients with panic disorder at perceiving their heartbeats? Biological Psychology, 42, 165–182. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Forkmann, T., Boecker, M., Wirtz, M., Eberle, N., Westhofen, M., Schauerte, P., … Norra, G. (2009). Development and validation of the Rasch-based depression screening (DESC) using Rasch analysis and structural equation modelling. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 40, 468–478. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Füstös, J., Gramann, K., Herbert, B. M. & Pollatos, O. (2013). On the embodiment of emotion regulation: Interoceptive awareness facilitates reappraisal. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8, 911–917. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Garfinkel, S. N., Barrett, A. B., Minati, L., Dolan, R. J., Seth, A. K. & Critchley, H. D. (2013). What the heart forgets: Cardiac timing influences memory for words and is modulated by metacognition and interoceptive sensitivity. Psychophysiology, 50, 505–512. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Garfinkel, S. N. & Critchley, H. D. (2013). Interoception, emotion and brain: new insights link internal physiology to social behaviour. Commentary on: “Anterior insular cortex mediates bodily sensibility and social anxiety” by Terasawa et al. (2012). Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8, 231–234. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Garfinkel, S. N., Seth, A. K., Barrett, A. B., Suzuki, K. & Critchley, H. D. (2015). Knowing your own heart: Distinguishing interoceptive accuracy from interoceptive awareness. Biological Psychology, 104, 65–74. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Grosche, C., Sütterlin, S., Koch, S., Schulz, S. & Vögele, C. (2014). Interoception and inhibitory capacity in panic disorder and somatoform disorders. In C. VögeleEd., 13. Kongress der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Verhaltensmedizin und Verhaltensmodifikation (pp. 7). Basel, Switzerland: Karger. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hayes, S. C., Luoma, J. B., Bond, F. W., Masuda, A. & Lillis, J. (2006). Acceptance and commitment therapy: Model, processes and outcomes. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1–25). First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Herbert, B. M., Blechert, J., Hautzinger, M., Matthias, E. & Herbert, C. (2013). Intuitive eating is associated with interoceptive sensitivity. Effects on body mass index. Appetite, 70, 22–30. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Herbert, B. M., Muth, E. R., Pollatos, O. & Herbert, C. (2012). Interoception across modalities: On the relationship between cardiac awareness and the sensitivity for gastric functions. Plos One, 7, e36646. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Herbert, B. M., Pollatos, O. & Schandry, R. (2007). Interoceptive sensitivity and emotion processing: An EEG study. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 65, 214–227. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Herbert, B. M., Ulbrich, P. & Schandry, R. (2007). Interoceptive sensitivity and physical effort: Implications for the self-control of physical load in everyday life. Psychophysiology, 44, 194–202. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • IBM. (2011). SPSS Statistics (Version 20) [Computer software]. Somers, NY: SPSS. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Jones, G. E. & Hollandsworth, J. G. (1981). Heart-rate discrimination before and after exercise-induced augmented cardiac activity. Psychophysiology, 18, 252–257. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Juslin, P., Winman, A. & Olsson, H. (2000). Naive empiricism and dogmatism in confidence research: A critical examination of the hard-easy effect. Psychological Review, 107, 384–396. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kaufmann, T., Vögele, C., Sütterlin, S., Lukito, S. & Kübler, A. (2012). Effects of resting heart rate variability on performance in the P300 brain-computer interface. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 83, 336–341. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kelemen, W. L., Frost, P. J. & Weaver, C. A. (2000). Individual differences in metacognition: Evidence against a general metacognitive ability. Memory & Cognition, 28, 92–107. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kessel, R., Gecht, J., Forkmann, T., Drueke, B., Gauggel, S. & Mainz, V. (2014). Metacognitive monitoring of attention performance and its influencing factors. Psychological Research – Psychologische Forschung, 78, 597–607. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Khalsa, S. S., Rudrauf, D., Damasio, A. R., Davidson, R. J., Lutz, A. & Tranel, D. (2008). Interoceptive awareness in experienced meditators. Psychophysiology, 45, 671–677. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Koriat, A., Bjork, R. A., Sheffer, L. & Bar, S. K. (2004). Predicting one’s own forgetting: The role of experience-based and theory-based processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology – General, 133, 643–656. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Koriat, A., Ma’ayan, H. & Nussinson, R. (2006). The intricate relationships between monitoring and control in metacognition: Lessons for the cause-and-effect relation between subjective experience and behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology – General, 135, 36–69. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ladegaard, N., Larsen, E. R., Videbech, P. & Lysaker, P. H. (2014). Higher-order social cognition in first-episode major depression. Psychiatry Research, 216, 37–43. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lysaker, P. H., Vohs, J., Hamm, J. A., Kukla, M., Minor, K. S., de Jong, S., … Dimaggio, G. (2014). Deficits in metacognitive capacity distinguish patients with schizophrenia from those with prolonged medical adversity. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 55, 126–132. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mcfarland, R. A. (1975). Heart-rate perception and heart-rate control. Psychophysiology, 12, 402–405. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mehling, W. E., Price, C., Daubenmier, J. J., Acree, M., Bartmess, E. & Stewart, A. (2012). The Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA). PloS One, 7, e48230. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Michal, M., Reuchlein, B., Adler, J., Reiner, I., Beutel, M. E., Vögele, C., … Schulz, A. (2014). Striking discrepancy of anomalous body experiences with normal interoceptive accuracy in depersonalization-derealization disorder. PLoS ONE, 9, e89823. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • O’Brien, W. H., Reid, G. J. & Jones, K. R. (1998). Differences in heartbeat awareness among males with higher and lower levels of systolic blood pressure. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 29, 53–63. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Parkin, L., Morgan, R., Rosselli, A., Howard, M., Sheppard, A., Evans, D., … Dunn, B. (2014). Exploring the relationship between mindfulness and cardiac perception. Mindfulness, 5, 298–313. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pollatos, O., Gramann, K. & Schandry, R. (2007). Neural systems connecting interoceptive awareness and feelings. Human Brain Mapping, 28, 9–18. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pollatos, O., Kurz, A. L., Albrecht, J., Schreder, T., Kleemann, A. M., Schöpf, V., … Schandry, R. (2008). Reduced perception of bodily signals in anorexia nervosa. Eating Behaviors, 9, 381–388. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pollatos, O., Matthias, E. & Schandry, R. (2007). Heartbeat perception and P300 amplitude in a visual oddball paradigm. Clinical Neurophysiology, 118, 2248–2253. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pollatos, O. & Schandry, R. (2008). Emotional processing and emotional memory are modulated by interoceptive awareness. Cognition & Emotion, 22, 272–287. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pollatos, O., Schandry, R., Auer, D. P. & Kaufmann, C. (2007). Brain structures mediating cardiovascular arousal and interoceptive awareness. Brain Research, 1141, 178–187. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pollatos, O., Traut-Mattausch, E. & Schandry, R. (2009). Differential effects of anxiety and depression on interoceptive accuracy. Depression and Anxiety, 26, 167–173. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pollatos, O., Yeldesbay, A., Pikovsky, A. & Rosenblum, M. (2014). How much time has passed? Ask your heart. Frontiers in Neurorobotics, 8, 1–9. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Porges, S. (1993). Body perception questionnaire: Laboratory of developmental assessment. Baltimore, MD: University of Maryland. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Rhodes, M. G. & Tauber, S. K. (2011). The influence of delaying judgments of learning on metacognitive accuracy: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 131–148. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schandry, R. (1981). Heart beat perception and emotional experience. Psychophysiology, 18, 483–488. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Scheyer, R., Reznik, N., Apter, A., Seidman, L. J. & Koren, D. (2014). Metacognition in non-psychotic help-seeking adolescents: Associations with prodromal symptoms, distress and psychosocial deterioration. The Israel Journal of Psychiatry and Related Sciences, 51, 34–43. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Soderstrom, N. C., Davalos, D. B. & Vazquez, S. M. (2011). Metacognition and depressive realism: Evidence for the level-of-depression account. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 16, 461–472. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R. & Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologist Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Sütterlin, S., Schulz, S. M., Stumpf, T., Pauli, P. & Vögele, C. (2013). Enhanced cardiac perception is associated with increased susceptibility to framing effects. Cognitive Science, 37, 922–935. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., Ridgeway, V. A., Soulsby, J. M. & Lau, M. A. (2000). Prevention of relapse/recurrence in major depression by mindfulness-based cognitive therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 615–623. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Terhaar, J., Viola, F. C., Bar, K. J. & Debener, S. (2012). Heartbeat evoked potentials mirror altered body perception in depressed patients. Clinical Neurophysiology, 123, 1950–1957. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • The Mathworks Inc. (R2012a). MATHLAB [Computer software]. Natick, MA: Mathworks Inc. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Vaitl, D. (1996). Interoception. Biological Psychology, 42, 1–27. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wells, A., Fisher, P., Myers, S., Wheatley, J., Patel, T. & Brewin, C. R. (2009). Metacognitive therapy in recurrent and persistent depression: A multiple-baseline study of a new treatment. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 33, 291–300. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Werner, N. S., Jung, K., Duschek, S. & Schandry, R. (2009). Enhanced cardiac perception is associated with benefits in decision-making. Psychophysiology, 46, 1123–1129. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Werner, N. S., Peres, I., Duschek, S. & Schandry, R. (2010). Implicit memory for emotional words is modulated by cardiac perception. Biological Psychology, 85, 370–376. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Whitehead, W. E., Drescher, V. M., Heiman, P. & Blackwell, B. (1977). Relation of heart-rate control to heartbeat perception. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 2, 371–392. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar