Skip to main content
Original Article

Beliefs About the Effects of Alcohol on the Personality of Oneself and Others

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1614-0001/a000084

The distinction between the effects people expect alcohol to have on themselves and on others is poorly understood. This study employs the Five Factor Model (FFM) of personality for investigating these two types of beliefs. Participants responded to short personality questionnaires with instructions to describe themselves and an average young man while moderately intoxicated; they also described their actual and desired personality trait levels. Intoxication was believed to decrease Conscientiousness and increase Neuroticism as well as Extraversion for actors as well as observers alike, while predictions for Openness and Agreeableness depended on the rating target. Profile similarity analysis revealed that, although both types of beliefs reflected socially undesirable trait levels in domains other than Extraversion, actor-expectations were less undesirable than observer-expectations. Implications of the findings suggest that the FFM can be profitably used to study intoxication-related beliefs.

References

  • Alicke, M. D. (1985). Global self-evaluation as determined by the desirability and controllability of trait adjectives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1621–1630. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Alicke, M. D. , Klotz, M. L. , Breitenbecher, D. L. , Yurak, T. J. , Vredenburg, D. S. (1995). Personal contact, individuation, and the better-than-average effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 804–825. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Allik, J. , McCrae, R. R. (2002). A five-factor theory perspective. In R. R. McCrae J. Allik, Eds., The Five-Factor Model of personality across cultures: International and cultural psychology (1st ed., pp. 303–323). New York: Springer. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Allik, J. , Mõttus, R. , Realo, A. (2010). Does national character reflect mean personality traits when both are measured by the same instrument? Journal of Research in Personality, 44(1), 62–69. doi:10.1016/j.jrp. 2009.10.008 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Barresi, J. , Moore, C. (1996). Intentional relations and social understanding. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 19(1), 107–129. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bleidorn, W. (2009). Linking personality states, current social roles and major life goals. European Journal of Personality, 23, 509–530. doi 10.1002/per.731 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cooper, M. L. (1994). Motivations for alcohol use among adolescents: Development and validation of a four-factor model. Psychological Assessment, 6, 117–128. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Costa, P. J. , Terracciano, A. , McCrae, R. R. (2001). Gender differences in personality traits across cultures: Robust and surprising findings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 322–331. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Costa, P. T. , McCrae, R. R. (1995). Domains and facets: Hierarchical personality assessment using the Revised NEO – Personality Inventory. Journal of Personality Assessment, 64, 21. doi 10.1207/s15327752jpa6401_2 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cox, W. M. , Klinger, E. (1988). A motivational model of alcohol use. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, Models of Addiction, 97, 168–180. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Davidson, R. J. , Lewis, D. A. , Alloy, L. B. , Amaral, D. G. , Bush, G. , Cohen, J. D. , ... Peterson, B. S. (2002). Neural and behavioral substrates of mood and mood regulation. Biological Psychiatry, 52, 478–502. doi 10.1016/S0006–3223(02)01458–0 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • DeYoung, C. G. (2010). Toward a theory of the big five. Psychological Inquiry, 21(1), 26. doi 10.1080/10478401003648674 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fleeson, W. (2001). Toward a structure- and process-integrated view of personality: Traits as density distributions of states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 1011–1027. doi 10.1037/0022-3514.80.6.1011 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Furr, R. M. (2008). A framework for profile similarity: Integrating similarity, normativeness, and distinctiveness. Journal of Personality, 76, 1267–1316. doi 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008. 00521.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Furr, R. M. (2010). The double-entry intraclass correlation as an index of profile similarity: Meaning, limitations, and alternatives. Journal of Personality Assessment, 92, 1–15. doi 10.1080/00223890903379134 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • George, W. H. , Dermen, K. H. (1988). Self-reported alcohol expectancies for self and other as a function of behavior type and dosage set. Journal of Substance Abuse, 1(1), 71–78. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Goldman, M. S. , Del Boca, F. K. , Darkes, J. (1999). Alcohol expectancy theory: The application of cognitive neuroscience. In H. Blane K. Leonard, Eds., Psychological theories of drinking and alcoholism. The Guilford substance abuse series (2nd ed., pp. 203–246). New York, London: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Heller, D. , Watson, D. , Komar, J. , Min, J. , Perunovic, W. Q. E. (2007). Contextualized personality: Traditional and new assessment procedures. Journal of Personality, 75, 1229–1254. doi 10.1111/j.1467–6494.2007.00474.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hoorens, V. (1993). Self-enhancement and superiority biases in social comparison. European Review of Social Psychology, 4, 113. doi 10.1080/14792779343000040 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Johnson, J. A. (1997). Units of analysis for the description and explanation of personality. In R. Hogan J. Johnson S. Briggs, Eds., Handbook of personality psychology (pp. 73–93). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jones, B. T. , Corbin, W. , Fromme, K. (2001). A review of expectancy theory and alcohol consumption. Addiction, 96(1), 57–72. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jones, E. E. , Nisbett, R. E. (1987). The actor and the observer: Divergent perceptions of the causes of behavior. In E. E. Jones D. E. Kanhouse H. H. Kelley R. E. Nisbett S. Valins B. Weiner, Eds., Attribution: Perceiving the causes of behavior (pp. 79–94). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Konstabel, K. , Aavik, T. , Allik, J. (2006). Social desirability and consensual validity of personality traits. European Journal of Personality, 20, 549–566. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Krueger, J. (1998). Enhancement bias in descriptions of self and others. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 505–516. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Leigh, B. C. (1987). Beliefs about the effects of alcohol on self and others. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 48, 467–475. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Leigh, B. C. , Stacy, A. W. (1993). Alcohol outcome expectancies: Scale construction and predictive utility in higher order confirmatory models. Psychological Assessment, 5, 216–229. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Malouff, J. M. , Thorsteinsson, E. B. , Rooke, S. E. , Schutte, N. S. (2007). Alcohol involvement and the Five-Factor Model of personality: A meta-analysis. Journal of Drug Education, 37, 277–294. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McCrae, R. R. , Costa, P. T. (2008). Empirical and theoretical status of the five-factor model of personality traits. In G. Boyle G. Matthews D. Saklofske, Eds., Sage handbook of personality theory and assessment (Vol. 1, pp. 273–294). Los Angeles, CA: Sage. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • McCrae, R. R. , John, O. P. (1992). An introduction to the Five-Factor Model and its applications. Journal of Personality, 60, 175–215. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McGraw, K. O. , Wong, S. P. (1996). Forming inferences about some intraclass correlation coefficients. Psychological Methods, 1(1), 30–46. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Moss, A. C. , Albery, I. P. (2009). A dual-process model of the alcohol–behavior link for social drinking. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 516–530. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mõttus, R. , Allik, J. , Pullmann, H. (2007). Does personality vary across ability levels? A study using self- and other-ratings. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 155–170. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mõttus, R. , Indus, K. , Allik, J. (2008). Accuracy of only children stereotype. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 1047–1052. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Realo, A. , Allik, J. , Lönnqvist, J. , Verkasalo, M. , Kwiatkowska, A. , Kööts, L. , ... Renge, V. (2009). Mechanisms of the national character stereotype: How people in six neighboring countries of Russia describe themselves and the typical Russian. European Journal of Personality, 23, 229–249. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Remington, N. A. , Fabrigar, L. R. , Visser, P. S. (2000). Reexamining the circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 286–300. doi 10.1037/0022-3514.79.2.286 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Room, R. (2001). Intoxication and bad behavior: Understanding cultural differences in the link. Social Science and Medicine, 53, 189–198. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tekkel, M. , Veideman, T. , Rahu, M. (2009). Health behavior among Estonian adult population, 2008. Tallinn, Estonia: National Institute for Health Development. Retrieved from www2.tai.ee/TAI/TKU2008.pdf First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Terracciano, A. , Abdel-Khalek, A. M. , Adam, N. , Adamovova, L. , Ahn, C. , Ahn, H. , ... Robert, R. (2005). National character does not reflect mean personality trait levels in 49 cultures. Science, 310(5745), 96–100. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Watson, D. (1982). The actor and the observer: How are their perceptions of causality divergent? Psychological Bulletin, 92, 682–700. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar