Skip to main content
Original Article

Warmer Hearts, Warmer Rooms

How Positive Communal Traits Increase Estimates of Ambient Temperature

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000147

Conceptual representations of warmth have been shown to be related to people’s perceptions of ambient temperature. Based on this premise, we hypothesized that merely thinking about personality traits related to communion (but not agency) influences physical experience of warmth. Specifically, the three studies revealed that (a) perceptions of temperature are influenced by both positive and negative attributes within the communion but not agency dimension, (b) the effect is stronger when traits indicate sociability rather than morality subdimension of communion, and (c) communion activation affects temperature perceptions independently of target’s or self-perceptions.

References

  • Abele, A. E. , Wojciszke, B. (2007). Agency and communion from the perspective of self versus others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 751–763. doi 10.1037/0022-3514.93.5.751 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Anderson, C. A. , & Sedikides, C. (1991). Contributions of a typological approach to associationistic and dimensional views of person perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 203–217. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Anderson, M. L. (2010). Neural reuse: A fundamental organizational principle of the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 245–313. doi 10.1017/S0140525X10000853 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Asch, S. E. (1946). Forming impressions of personality. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 41, 258–290. doi 10.1037/h0055756 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Asch, S. E. , Zukier, H. (1984). Thinking about persons. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 1230–1240. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bakan, D. (1966). The duality of human existence: An essay on psychology and religion. Chicago, IL: Rand McNally. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bargh, J. , Shalev, I. (2012). The substitutability of physical and social warmth in daily life. Emotion, 12(1), 154–162. doi 10.1037/a0023527 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577–660. doi 10.1.1.4.5511 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Barsalou, L. W. (2008a). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617–645. doi 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Barsalou, L. W. (2008b). Grounding symbolic operations in the brain’s modal systems. In G. R. Semin, E. R. Smith, (Eds.), Embodied grounding: Social, cognitive, affective, and neuroscientific approaches (pp. 9–42). New York: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bem, S. L. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42, 155–162. doi 10.1037/h0036215 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss. London: Hogarth. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Brambilla, M. , Rusconi, P. , Sacchi, S. , Cherubini, P. (2011). Looking for honesty: The primary role of morality (vs. sociability and competence) in information gathering. European Journal of Social Psychology, 41, 135–143. doi 10.1002/ejsp.744 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bullock, J. G. , Green, D. P. , Ha, S. E. (2010). Yes, but what’s the mechanism? (Don’t expect an easy answer). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 550–558. doi 10.1037/a0018933 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Caporael, L. R. (1997). The evolution of truly social cognition: The core configurations model. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4, 276–298. doi 10.1207/s15327957pspr0104_1 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chandler, J. , Reinhard, D. , Schwarz, N. (in press). To judge a book by its weight you need to know its content: Knowledge moderates the use of embodied cues. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Chandler, J. , Schwarz, N. (2009). How extending your middle finger affects your perception of others: Body movements as primes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 123–128. doi 10.1016/j.jesp.2008.06.012 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cuddy, A. J. C. , Fiske, S. T. , Glick, P. (2008). Warmth and competence as universal dimensions of social perception: The Stereotype Content Model and the BIAS Map. In M. P. Zanna, (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 40, pp. 61–149). New York: Academic Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Efron, B. (1987). “Better bootstrap confidence intervals.” Journal of the American Statistical Association, 82, 171–185. doi 10.2307/2289144 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Eriksson, M. , Lundeberg, T. , Uvnäs-Moberg, K. (1996). Studies on cutaneous blood flow in the mammary gland of lactating rats. Acta Physiologica Scandinavica, 158, 1–6. doi 10.1046/j.1365-201X.1996.487226000.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fiske, A. P. (1991). Structures of social life: The four elementary forms of human relations. New York: Free Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Fiske, S. T. , Cuddy, A. J. C. , Glick, P. (2007). Universal dimensions of social cognition: Warmth and competence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 77–83. doi 10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.005 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gawronski, B. , & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2011). The associative-propositional evaluation model: Theory, evidence, and open questions. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 59–127. doi 10.1016/B978-0-12-385522-0.00002-0 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Giessner, S. R. , & Schubert, T. W. (2007). High in the hierarchy: How vertical location and judgments of leaders’ power are interrelated. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 104, 30–44. doi 10.1016/j.obhdp.2006.10.001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Glucksberg, S. , McGlone, M. S. , Manfredi, D. A. (1997). Property attribution in metaphor comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 36, 50–67. doi 10.1006/jmla.1996.2479 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Harlow, H. F. (1957). Experimental analysis of behavior. American Psychologist, 12, 485–490. doi 10.1037/h0047351 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hauk, O. , Johnsrude, I. , Pulvermüller, F. (2004). Somatotopic representation of action words in human motor and premotor cortex. Neuron, 41, 301–307. doi 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00838-9 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • IJzerman, H. , Cohen, D. (2011). Grounding cultural syndromes: Body comportment and values in honor and dignity cultures. European Journal of Social Psychology, 41, 456–467. doi 10.1002/ejsp.806 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • IJzerman, H. , Gallucci, M. , Pouw, W. T. J. L. , Weissgerber, C. S. , Van Doesum, N. J. , Williams, K. D. (in press). Cold-blood loneliness: Social exclusion leads to lower skin temperatures. Acta Psychologica. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • IJzerman, H. , Karremans, J. C. , Thomsen, L. , Schubert, T. W. (2013). Caring for sharing: How attachment styles modulate cues of physical warmth. Social Psychology, 44, 161–166. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • IJzerman, H. , Koole, S. L. (2011). From perceptual rags to metaphoric riches – bodily, social, and cultural constraints on sociocognitive metaphors: Comment on Landau, Meier, and Keefer (2010). Psychological Bulletin, 137, 355–361. doi 10.1037/a0022373 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • IJzerman, H. , Semin, G. R. (2009). The thermometer of social relations: mapping social proximity on temperature. Psychological Science, 20, 1214–1220. doi 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02434.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • IJzerman, H. , Semin, G. R. (2010). Temperature perceptions as a ground for social proximity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 867–873. doi 10.1016/j.jesp.2010.07.015 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lakoff, G. , Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Lakoff, G. , Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and it’s challenge to Western thought. New York: HarperCollins. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Landau, M. J. , Meier, B. P. , Keefer, L. A. (2010). A metaphor-enriched social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 1045–1067. doi 10.1037/a0020970 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Leach, C. W. , Ellemers, N. , Barreto, M. (2007). Group virtue: The importance of morality (vs. competence and sociability) in the positive evaluation of in-groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 234–249. doi 10.1037/0022-3514.93.2.234 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Meier, B. P. , Schnall, S. , Schwarz, N. , Bargh, J. A. (in press). Embodiment in social psychology. Topics in Cognitive Science. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Nisbett, R. , Wilson, T. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Osgood, C. E. , Suci, G. J. , Tannenbaum, P. H. (1957). The measurement of meaning. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Peeters, G. (1992). Evaluative meanings of adjectives in vitro and in context: Some theoretical implications and practical consequences of positive-negative asymmetry and behavioral-adaptive concepts of evaluation. Psychologica Belgica, 32, 211–231. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Preacher, K. J. , & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879–891. doi 10.3758/BRM.40.3.879 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rai, T. S. , Fiske, A. P. (2011). Moral psychology is relationship regulation: Moral motives for unity, hierarchy, equality, and proportionality. Psychological Review, 118, 57–75. doi 10.1037/ a0021867 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Reeder, G. D. , Pryor, J. B. , Wojciszke, B. (1992). Trait-behavior relations in social information processing. In G. Semin, K. Fiedler, (Eds.), Language and social cognition (pp. 37–57). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Santiago, J. , Román, A. , Ouellet, M. (2011). Flexible foundations of abstract thought: A review and a theory. In A. Maass, T. Schubert, (Eds.), Spatial dimensions of social thought (pp. 39–108). Leipzig, Germany: de Gruyter Mouton. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sherman, G. D. , & Clore, G. L. (2009). The color of sin: White and black are perceptual symbols of moral purity and pollution. Psychological Science, 20, 1019–1025. doi 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02403.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Simmons, K. , Barsalou, L. W. (2003). The similarity-in-topography principle: Reconciling theories of conceptual deficits. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20, 451–486. doi 10.1080/02643290342000032 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Skowronski, J. , & Carlston, D. (1987). Social judgment and social memory: The role of cue diagnosticity in negativity, positivity, and extremity biases. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 689–699. doi 10.1037//0022-3514.52.4.689 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Smith, E. R. (1996). What do connectionism and social psychology offer each other? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 893–912. doi 10.1037//0022-3514.70.5.893 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Topolinski, S. (2011). I 5683 you: Dialing phone numbers on cell phones activates key-concordant concepts. Psychological Science, 22, 355–360. doi 10.1177/0956797610397668 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Uvnäs-Moberg, K. (1998). Oxytocin may mediate the benefits of positive social interaction and emotions. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 23, 819–835. doi 10.1016/S0306-4530(98) 00056-0 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vacharkulksemsuk, T. , Fredrickson, B. L. (2011). Strangers in sync: Achieving embodied rapport through shared movements. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 399–402. doi 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.07.015 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Williams, J. E. , & Best, D. L. (1990). Sex and psyche: Gender and self viewed cross-culturally. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Williams, L. E. , & Bargh, J. A. (2008). Experiencing physical warmth promotes interpersonal warmth. Science, 322, 606–607. doi 10.1126/science.1162548 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Williams, L. E. , Huang, J. Y. , Bargh, J. A. (2009). The scaffolded mind: Higher mental processes are grounded in early experience of the physical world. European Journal of Social Psychology, 39, 1257–1267. doi 10.1002/ejsp.665 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wojciszke, B. (1994). Multiple meanings of behavior: Construing actions in terms of competence or morality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 222–232. doi 10.1037//0022-3514.67.2.222 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wojciszke, B. , & Abele, A. E. (2008). The primacy of communion over agency and its reversals in evaluations. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 1139–1147. doi 10.1002/ejsp.549 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zhong, C. B. , Leonardelli, G. J. (2008). Cold and lonely: Does social exclusion literally feel cold? Psychological Science, 19, 838–842. doi 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02165.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zhong, C. B. , Liljenquist, K. (2006). Washing away your sins: Threatened morality and physical cleansing. Science, 313, 1451–1452. doi 10.1126/science.1130726 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar