Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000259

Abstract. We investigated the moderation of effects of activated gender stereotypes on the performance of women in tests of different ability domains. The Bem Sex Role Inventory assessed masculinity and femininity. The difference of the masculinity and femininity scores served as a continuous independent variable of gender role orientation. Only participants with feminine gender role orientation suffered from stereotype activation with regard to mental rotation and math performance. In contrast, participants with feminine gender role orientation profited from stereotype activation with regard to emotional sensitivity performance. These results demonstrate a generally higher susceptibility to gender stereotypes of women with feminine gender role orientation. Higher self-relevance of stereotypes was associated with stronger stereotype threat, but also stronger stereotype lift or boost.

References

  • Ambady, N., Paik, S., Steele, J., Owen-Smith, A. & Mitchell, J. (2004). Deflecting negative self-relevant stereotype activation: The effects of individuation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 401–408. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2003.08.003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Amthauer, R., Brocke, B., Liepmann, D. & Beauducel, A. (1999). Intelligenz-Struktur-Test 2000: IST 2000 [Intelligence Structure Test 2000]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Aronson, J., Lustina, M. J., Good, C., Keough, K., Steele, C. M. & Brown, J. (1999). When white men can’t do math: Necessary and sufficient factors in stereotype threat. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 29–46. doi: 10.1006/jesp.1998.1371 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bem, S. (1981). Bem Sex-Role Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bergeron, D. M., Block, C. J. & Echtenkamp, B. A. (2006). Disabling the able: Stereotype threat and women’s work performance. Human Performance, 19, 133–158. doi: 10.1207/s15327043hup1902_3 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Blanchard-Fields, F., Suhrer-Roussel, L. & Hertzog, C. (1994). A confirmatory factor analysis of the Bem Sex Role Inventory: Old questions, new answers. Sex Roles, 30, 423–457. doi: 10.1007/BF01420602 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brodish, A. B. & Devine, P. G. (2009). The role of performance-avoidance goals and worry in mediating the relationship between stereotype threat and performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 180–185. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2008.08.005 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brown, R. P. & Pinel, E. C. (2003). Stigma on my mind: Individual differences in the experience of stereotype threat. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 626–633. doi: 10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00039-8 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cadinu, M., Maass, A., Rosabianca, A. & Kiesner, J. (2005). Why do women underperform under stereotype threat? Psychological Science, 16, 572–578. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01577.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chalabaev, A., Sarrazin, P. & Fontayne, P. (2009). Stereotype endorsement and perceived ability as mediators of the girls’ gender orientation-soccer performance relationship. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 10, 297–299. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2008.08.002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cohen, G. L., Garcia, J., Apfel, N. & Master, A. (2006). Reducing the racial achievement gap: A social-psychological intervention. Science, 313, 1307–1310. doi: 10.1126/science.1128317 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Crisp, R. J., Bache, L. M. & Maitner, A. T. (2009). Dynamics of social comparison in counter-stereotypic domains: Stereotype boost, not stereotype threat, for women engineering majors. Social Influence, 4, 171–184. doi: 10.1080/15534510802607953 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Davies, P. G., Spencer, S. J., Quinn, D. M. & Gerhardstein, R. (2002). Consuming images: How television commercials that elicit stereotype threat can restrain women academically and professionally. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1615–1628. doi: 10.1177/014616702237644 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dijksterhuis, A. & Bargh, J. A. (2001). The perception-behavior expressway: Automatic effects of social perception on social behavior. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 1–40. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dodonova, Y. A. & Dodonov, Y. S. (2012). Speed of emotional information processing and emotional intelligence. International Journal of Psychology, 47, 429–437. doi: 10.1080/00207594.2012.656131 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ekman, P. & Friesen, W. V. (1975). Unmasking the face: A guide to recognizing emotion from facial cues. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Halpern, D. F. (1997). Sex differences in intelligence. The American Psychologist, 52, 1091–1102. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.52.10.1091 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Heilman, M. E. (2001). Description and prescription: How gender stereotypes prevent women’s ascent up the organizational ladder. Journal of Social Issues, 57, 657–674. doi: 10.1111/0022-4537.00234 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Irtel, H. (2007). PXLab: The Psychological Experiments Laboratory [online]. Version 2.1.11. Mannheim, Germany: University of Mannheim. Retrieved from http://www.pxlab.de First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Jost, J. T. & Banaji, M. R. (1994). The role of stereotyping in system justification and the production of false consciousness. The British Journal of Social Psychology, 22, 1–27. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8309.1994.tb01008.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Juni, S., Rahamim, E. L. & Brannon, R. (1985). Sex role development as a function of parent models and oedipal fixation. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 146, 89–99. doi: 10.1080/00221325.1985.9923451 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Krampen, G., Effertz, B., Jostock, U. & Müller, B. (1990). Gender differences in personality: Biological and/or psychological? European Journal of Personality, 4, 303–317. doi: 10.1002/per.2410040404 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Martens, A., Johns, M., Greenberg, J. & Schimel, J. (2006). Combating stereotype threat: The effect of self-affirmation on women’s intellectual performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 236–243. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2005.04.010 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Massa, L. J., Mayer, R. E. & Bohon, L. M. (2005). Individual differences in gender role belief influence spatial ability test performance. Learning and Individual Differences, 15, 99–111. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2004.11.002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McGlone, M. S. & Aronson, J. (2006). Stereotype threat, identity salience, and spatial reasoning. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 27, 486–493. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2006.06.003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nguyen, H.-H. D. & Ryan, A. M. (2008). Does stereotype threat affect test performance of minorities and women? A meta-analysis of experimental evidence. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93, 1314–1334. doi: 10.1037/a0012702 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ortner, T. M. & Sieverding, M. (2008). Where are the gender differences? Male priming boosts spatial skills in women. Sex Roles, 59, 274–281. doi: 10.1007/s11199-008-9448-9 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schmader, T. (2002). Gender identification moderates stereotype threat effects on women’s math performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 194–201. doi: 10.1006/jesp.2001.1500 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schmader, T. & Johns, M. (2003). Converging evidence that stereotype threat reduces working memory capacity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 440–452. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.3.440 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schmader, T., Johns, M. & Barquissau, M. (2004). The cost of accepting gender differences: The role of stereotype endorsement in women’s experience in the math domain. Sex Roles, 50, 835–850. doi: 10.1023/B:SERS.0000029101.74557.a0 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schmader, T., Johns, M. & Forbes, C. (2008). An integrated process model of stereotype threat effects on performance. Psychological Review, 115, 336–356. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.115.2.336 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schneider-Düker, M. & Kohler, A. (1988). Die Erfassung von Geschlechtsrollen – Ergebnisse zur deutschen Neukonstruktion des Bem Sex Role Inventory [Assessment of gender roles: Results for the new German version of the Bem Sex-Role Inventory]. Diagnostica, 34, 256–270. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Seibt, B. & Förster, J. (2004). Stereotype threat and performance: How self-stereotypes influence processing by inducing regulatory foci. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 38. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Shepard, R. N. & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science, 171, 701–703. doi: 10.1126/science.171.3972.701 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Shih, M., Ambady, N., Richeson, J. A., Fujita, K. & Gray, H. M. (2002). Stereotype performance boosts: The impact of self-relevance and the manner of stereotype activation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 638–647. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.83.3.638 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Shih, M. J., Pittinsky, T. L. & Ho, G. C. (2012). Stereotype boost. In M. InzlichtT. SchmaderEds., Stereotype threat: Theory, process, and application (pp. 141–156). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Spencer, S. J., Steele, C. M. & Quinn, D. M. (1999). Stereotype threat and women’s math performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 4–28. doi: 10.1006/jesp.1998.1373 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stone, J., Lynch, C., Sjomeling, M. & Darley, J. M. (1999). Stereotype threat effects on Black and White athletic performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1213–1227. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.77.6.1213 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tempel, T. & Neumann, R. (2014). Stereotype threat, test anxiety, and mathematics performance. Social Psychology of Education, 17, 491–501. doi: 10.1007/s11218-014-9263-9 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vandenberg, S. G. & Kuse, A. R. (1978). Mental rotations, a group test of three-dimensional spatial visualizations. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 39, 456–467. doi: 10.2466/pms.1978.47.2.599 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Voracek, M., Pietschnig, J., Nader, I. W. & Stieger, S. (2011). Digit ratio (2D:4D) and sex-role orientation: Further evidence and meta-analysis. Personality and Individual Differences, 51, 417–422. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.06.009 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Walton, G. M. & Cohen, G. L. (2003). Stereotype lift. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 456–467. doi: 10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00019-2 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Weekes, N. Y., Zaidel, D. W. & Zaidel, E. (1995). Effects of sex and sex role attributions on the ear advantage in dichotic listening. Neuropsychology, 9, 62–67. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.9.1.62 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar