Skip to main content
Free AccessOriginal Article

“I Know Things They Don’t Know!”

The Role of Need for Uniqueness in Belief in Conspiracy Theories

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000306

Abstract. In the current research, we investigated whether belief in conspiracy theories satisfies people’s need for uniqueness. We found that the tendency to believe in conspiracy theories was associated with the feeling of possessing scarce information about the situations explained by the conspiracy theories (Study 1) and higher need for uniqueness (Study 2). Further two studies using two different manipulations of need for uniqueness (Studies 3 and 4) showed that people in a high need for uniqueness condition displayed higher conspiracy belief than people in a low need for uniqueness condition. This conclusion is strengthened by a small-scale meta-analysis. These studies suggest that conspiracy theories may serve people’s desire to be unique, highlighting a motivational underpinning of conspiracy belief.

References

  • Abalakina-Paap, M., Stephan, W. G., Craig, T. & Gregory, W. L. (1999). Beliefs in conspiracies. Political Psychology, 20, 637–647. doi: 10.1111/0162-895X.00160 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Abelson, R. P. (1986). Beliefs are like possessions. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 16, 223–250. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-5914.1986.tb00078.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Aust, F., Diedenhofen, B., Ullrich, S. & Musch, J. (2013). Seriousness checks are useful to improve data validity in online research. Behavior Research Methods, 45, 527–535. doi: 10.3758/s13428-012-0265-2 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Billig, M. (1987). Anti-semitic themes and the British far left: Some social-psychological observations on indirect aspects of the conspiracy tradition. In C. F. GraumannS. MoscoviciEds., Changing conceptions of conspiracy (pp. 115–136). New York, NY: Springer. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T. & Rothstein, H. R. (2009). Introduction to meta-analysis. Chichester, UK: Wiley. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bost, P. R. & Prunier, S. G. (2013). Rationality in conspiracy beliefs: The role of perceived motive. Psychological Reports, 113, 118–128. doi: 10.2466/17.04/PR0.113x17z0 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bost, P. R., Prunier, S. G. & Piper, A. J. (2010). Relations of familiarity with reasoning strategies in conspiracy beliefs. Psychological Reports, 107, 593–602. doi: 10.2466/07.09.17.PR0.107.5.593-602 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brewer, M. B. (1991). The social self: On being the same and different at the same time. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17, 475–482. doi: 10.1177/0146167291175001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brock, T. C. (1968). Implications of commodity theory for value change. In A. G. GreenwaldT. C. BrockT. M. OstromEds., Psychological foundations of attitudes (pp. 243–275). New York, NY: Academic Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Brock, T. C. & Brannon, L. A. (1992). Liberalization of commodity theory. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 13, 135–144. doi: 10.1207/s15324834basp1301_11 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brotherton, R. & French, C. C. (2014). Belief in conspiracy theories and susceptibility to the conjunction fallacy. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28, 238–248. doi: 10.1002/acp.2995 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brotherton, R. & French, C. C. (2015). Intention seekers: Conspiracist ideation and biased attributions of intentionality. PLoS One, 10, e0124125. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0124125 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brotherton, R., French, C. C. & Pickering, A. D. (2013). Measuring belief in conspiracy theories: The generic conspiracist beliefs scale. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 279. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00279 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bruder, M., Haffke, P., Neave, N., Nouripanah, N. & Imhoff, R. (2013). Measuring individual differences in generic beliefs in conspiracy theories across cultures: Conspiracy mentality questionnaire. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 225. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00225 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Butler, J. C. (2000). Personality and emotional correlates of right-wing authoritarianism. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 28, 1–14. doi: 10.2224/sbp.2000.28.1.1 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Butler, L. D., Koopman, C. & Zimbardo, P. G. (1995). The psychological impact of viewing the film “JFK”: Emotions, beliefs, and political behavioral intentions. Political Psychology, 16, 237–257. doi: 10.2307/3791831 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cheema, A. & Kaikati, A. M. (2010). The effect of need for uniqueness on word of mouth. Journal of Marketing Research, 47, 553–563. doi: 10.1509/jmkr.47.3.553 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cichocka, A., Marchlewska, M. & Golec de Zavala, A. (2016). Does self-love or self-hate predict conspiracy beliefs? Narcissism, self-esteem and the endorsement of conspiracy theories. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 7, 157–166. doi: 10.1177/1948550615616170 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155–159. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cumming, G. (2014). The new statistics: Why and how. Psychological Science, 25, 7–29. doi: 10.1177/0956797613504966 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Douglas, K. M. & Leite, A. C. (2008). Suspicion in the workplace: Organizational conspiracy theories and work-related outcomes. British Journal of Psychology, in press. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12212 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Douglas, K. M. & Sutton, R. M. The hidden impact of conspiracy theories: Perceived and actual influence of theories surrounding the death of Princess Diana. The Journal of Social Psychology, 148, 210–222. doi: 10.3200/SOCP.148.2.210-222 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Douglas, K. M. & Sutton, R. M. (2011). Does it take one to know one? Endorsement of conspiracy theories is influenced by personal willingness to conspire. The British Journal of Social Psychology, 50, 544–552. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8309.2010.02018.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Douglas, K. M. & Sutton, R. M. (2015). Climate change: Why the conspiracy theories are dangerous. Bulletin of the Atomic Scientists, 71, 98–106. doi: 10.1177/0096340215571908 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Douglas, K. M., Sutton, R. M., Callan, M. J., Dawtry, R. J. & Harvey, A. J. (2016). Someone is pulling the strings: Hypersensitive agency detection and belief in conspiracy theories. Thinking & Reasoning, 22, 57–77. doi: 10.1080/13546783.2015.1051586 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Emmons, R. A. (1984). Factor analysis and construct validity of the narcissistic personality inventory. Journal of Personality Assessment, 48, 291–300. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa4803_11 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fayant, M.-P., Sigall, H., Lemonnier, A., Retsin, E. & Alexopoulos, T. (in press). On the limitation of manipulation checks: An obstacle toward cumulative science. International Review of Social Psychology. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ferrand, L. & Ayora, P. (2009). Psychologie cognitive de la lecture [Cognitive psychology of reading]. Bruxelles, Belgium: De Boeck. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Fromkin, H. L. (1970). Effects of experimentally aroused feelings of indistinctiveness upon valuation of scarce and novel experiences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16, 521–529. doi: 10.1037/h0030059 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Funke, F. (2005). The dimensionality of right-wing authoritarianism: Lessons from the dilemma between theory and measurement. Political Psychology, 26, 195–218. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9221.2005.00415.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Goertzel, T. (1994). Belief in conspiracy theories. Political Psychology, 15, 731–742. doi: 10.2307/3791630 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gray, M. (2010). Revisiting Saddam Hussein’s political language: The sources and roles of conspiracy theories. Arab Studies Quarterly, 32, 28–46. Retrieved from https://www.jstor.org/stable/41858602 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Grzesiak-Feldman, M. (2015). Are the high authoritarians more prone to adopt conspiracy theories? The role of right-wing authoritarianism in conspiratorial thinking. In M. BilewiczA. CichockaW. SoralEds., The psychology of conspiracy (pp. 99–121). New York, NY: Routledge. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Grzesiak-Feldman, M. & Irzycka, M. (2009). Right-wing authoritarianism and conspiracy thinking in a polish sample. Psychological Reports, 105, 389–393. doi: 10.2466/PR0.105.2.389-393 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Higgins, J. P., Thompson, S. G., Deeks, J. J. & Altman, D. G. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analysis. British Medical Journal, 327, 557–560. doi: 10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Imhoff, R. & Lamberty, P. (in press). Too special to be duped: Need for uniqueness motivates conspiracy beliefs. European Journal of Social Psychology. doi: 10.1002/EJSP.2265 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jolley, D. & Douglas, K. M. (2014a). The effects of anti-vaccine conspiracy theories on vaccination intentions. PLoS One, 9, e89177. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0089177 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jolley, D. & Douglas, K. M. (2014b). The social consequences of conspiracism: Exposure to conspiracy theories decreases intentions to engage in politics and to reduce one’s carbon footprint. British Journal of Psychology, 105, 35–56. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12018 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Judd, C. M., McClelland, G. H. & Ryan, C. S. (2008). Data analysis: A model comparison approach (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Routledge. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Judd, C. M., Yzerbyt, V. Y. & Muller, D. (2014). Mediation and moderation. In H. ReisC. M. JuddEds., Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology (2nd ed., pp. 653–676). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Keeley, B. L. (1999). Of conspiracy theories. The Journal of Philosophy, 96, 109–126. doi: 10.2307/2564659 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kim, H. & Markus, H. R. (1999). Deviance or uniqueness, harmony or conformity? A cultural analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 785–800. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.77.4.785 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kim, H. S. & Drolet, A. (2003). Choice and self-expression: A cultural analysis of variety-seeking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 373–382. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.373 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kunda, Z. & Sanitioso, R. (1989). Motivated changes in the self-concept. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 25, 272–285. doi: 10.1016/0022-1031(89)90023-1 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lakoff, G. & Johnson, M. (1980/2002). Metaphors we live by. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Laland, K. N. (2004). Social learning strategies. Animal Learning & Behavior, 32, 4–14. doi: 10.3758/BF03196002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lantian, A. (2015). Rôle fonctionnel de l’adhésion aux théories du complot : un moyen de distinction ? [The functional role of believing in conspiracy theories: A way to distinguish ourselves from others?] (Doctoral dissertation, Université Grenoble Alpes). Retrieved from https://tel.archives-ouvertes.fr/tel-01251554/document First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Lantian, A., Muller, D., Nurra, C. & Douglas, K. M. (2016). Measuring belief in conspiracy theories: Validation of a French and English single-item scale. International Review of Social Psychology, 29, 1–14. doi: 10.5334/irsp.8 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lewandowsky, S., Oberauer, K. & Gignac, G. E. (2013). NASA faked the moon landing – Therefore, (climate) science is a hoax: An anatomy of the motivated rejection of science. Psychological Science, 24, 622–633. doi: 10.1177/0956797612457686 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lynn, M. (1991). Scarcity effects on value: A quantitative review of the commodity theory literature. Psychology & Marketing, 8, 43–57. doi: 10.1002/mar.4220080105 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lynn, M. & Harris, J. (1997). The desire for unique consumer products: A new individual differences scale. Psychology & Marketing, 14, 601–616. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1520-6793(199709)14:6<601::AID-MAR5>3.0.CO;2-B First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lynn, M. & Snyder, C. R. (2002). Uniqueness seeking. In C. R. SnyderS. J. LopezEds., Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 395–419). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Maimaran, M. & Wheeler, S. C. (2008). Circles, squares, and choice: The effect of shape arrays on uniqueness and variety seeking. Journal of Marketing Research, 45, 731–740. doi: 10.1509/jmkr.45.6.731 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Markus, H. R. & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224–253. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mason, F. (2002). A poor person’s cognitive mapping. In P. KnightEd., Conspiracy nation: The politics of paranoia in postwar America (pp. 40–56). New York, NY: New York University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Muller, D., Yzerbyt, V. Y. & Judd, C. M. (2008). Adjusting for a mediator in models with two crossed treatment variables. Organizational Research Methods, 11, 224–240. doi: 10.1177/1094428106296636 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Newheiser, A. K., Farias, M. & Tausch, N. (2011). The functional nature of conspiracy beliefs: Examining the underpinnings of belief in the Da Vinci Code conspiracy. Personality and Individual Differences, 51, 1007–1011. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.08.011 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Oliver, J. E. & Wood, T. J. (2014). Medical conspiracy theories and health behaviors in the United States. JAMA Internal Medicine, 174, 817–818. doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2014.190 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Passini, S. (2008). Exploring the multidimensional facets of authoritarianism: Authoritarian aggression and social dominance orientation. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 67, 51–60. doi: 10.1024/1421-0185.67.1.51 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Powell, F. A. (1974). The perception of self-uniqueness as a determinant of message choice and valuation. Speech Monographs, 41, 163–168. doi: 10.1080/03637757409375831 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Richard, F. D., Bond, C. F. Jr. & Stokes-Zoota, J. J. (2003). One hundred years of social psychology quantitatively described. Review of General Psychology, 7, 331–363. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.7.4.331 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sanitioso, R., Kunda, Z. & Fong, G. T. (1990). Motivated recruitment of autobiographical memories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 229–241. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.59.2.229 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sapountzis, A. & Condor, S. (2013). Conspiracy accounts as intergroup theories: Challenging dominant understandings of social power and political legitimacy. Political Psychology, 34, 731–752. doi: 10.1111/pops.12015 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schwarzer, G. (2007). Meta: An R package for meta-analysis. R News, 7, 40–45. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Sigall, H. & Mills, J. (1998). Measures of independent variables and mediators are useful in social psychology experiments: But are they necessary? Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2, 218–226. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0203_5 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Simmons, J. P., Nelson, L. D. & Simonsohn, U. (2012). A 21 word solution. Dialogue: The Official Newsletter of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, 26, 4–7. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Şimşek, Ö. F. & Yalınçetin, B. (2010). I feel unique, therefore I am: The development and preliminary validation of the personal sense of uniqueness (PSU) scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 576–581. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.05.006 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Snyder, C. R. (1992). Product scarcity by need for uniqueness interaction: A consumer catch-22 carousel? Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 13, 9–24. doi: 10.1207/s15324834basp1301_3 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Snyder, C. R. & Fromkin, H. L. (1977). Abnormality as a positive characteristic: The development and validation of a scale measuring need for uniqueness. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 86, 518–527. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.86.5.518 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Snyder, C. R. & Fromkin, H. L. (1980). Uniqueness: The pursuit of human difference. New York, NY: Springer. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stern, C., West, T. V. & Schmitt, P. G. (2014). The liberal illusion of uniqueness. Psychological Science, 25, 137–144. doi: 10.1177/0956797613500796 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Swami, V., Chamorro-Premuzic, T. & Furnham, A. (2010). Unanswered questions: A preliminary investigation of personality and individual difference predictors of 9/11 conspiracist beliefs. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24, 749–761. doi: 10.1002/acp.1583 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Swami, V., Coles, R., Stieger, S., Pietschnig, J., Furnham, A., Rehim, S. & Voracek, M. (2011). Conspiracist ideation in Britain and Austria: Evidence of a monological belief system and associations between individual psychological differences and real-world and fictitious conspiracy theories. British Journal of Psychology, 102, 443–463. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.2010.02004.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Swami, V. & Furnham, A. (2014). Political paranoia and conspiracy theories. In J.-P. ProoijenP. A. M. van LangeEds., Power politics, and paranoia: Why people are suspicious of their leaders (pp. 218–236). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Swami, V., Voracek, M., Stieger, S., Tran, U. S. & Furnham, A. (2014). Analytic thinking reduces belief in conspiracy theories. Cognition, 133, 572–585. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.08.006 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Takemura, K. (2014). Being different leads to being connected: On the adaptive function of uniqueness in “open” societies. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 45, 1579–1593. doi: 10.1177/0022022114548684 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Taylor, S. E. (1965). Eye movements in reading: Facts and fallacies. American Educational Research Journal, 2, 187–202. doi: 10.3102/00028312002004187 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tian, K. T., Bearden, W. O. & Hunter, G. L. (2001). Consumers’ need for uniqueness: Scale development and validation. Journal of Consumer Research, 28, 50–66. doi: 10.1086/321947 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Turner, J. C., Oakes, P. J., Haslam, S. A. & McGarty, C. (1994). Self and collective: Cognition and social context. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 454–463. doi: 10.1177/0146167294205002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Turner, J. C. & Reynolds, K. J. (2011). Self-categorization theory. In P. A. M. Van LangeA. W. KruglanskiE. T. HigginsEds., Handbook of theories in social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 399–417). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Uscinski, J. E., Klofstad, C. & Atkinson, M. D. (2016). What drives conspiratorial beliefs? The role of informational cues and predispositions. Political Research Quarterly, 69, 57–71. doi: 10.1177/1065912915621621 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • van der Linden, S. (2015). The conspiracy-effect: Exposure to conspiracy theories (about global warming) decreases pro-social behavior and science acceptance. Personality and Individual Differences, 87, 171–173. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.07.045 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • van Harreveld, F., Rutjens, B. T., Schneider, I. K., Nohlen, H. U. & Keskinis, K. (2014). In doubt and disorderly: Ambivalence promotes compensatory perceptions of order. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 1666–1676. doi: 10.1037/a0036099 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • van Prooijen, J.-W. & Acker, M. (2015). The influence of control on belief in conspiracy theories: Conceptual and applied extensions. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 29, 753–761. doi: 10.1002/acp.3161 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • van Prooijen, J.-W. & Jostmann, N. B. (2013). Belief in conspiracy theories: The influence of uncertainty and perceived morality. European Journal of Social Psychology, 43, 109–115. doi: 10.1002.ejsp.1922 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • van Prooijen, J.-W. & van Dijk, E. (2014). When consequence size predicts belief in conspiracy theories: The moderating role of perspective taking. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 55, 63–73. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2014.06.006 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wagenmakers, E. J., Wetzels, R., Borsboom, D., van der Maas, H. L. J. & Kievit, R. A. (2012). An agenda for purely confirmatory research. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 632–638. doi: 10.1177/1745691612463078 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wagner-Egger, P. & Bangerter, A. (2007). La vérité est ailleurs: Corrélats de l’adhésion aux théories du complot [The truth lies elsewhere: Correlates of belief in conspiracy theories]. Revue Internationale de Psychologie Sociale, 20, 31–61. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Whitson, J. A. & Galinsky, A. D. (2008). Lacking control increases illusory pattern perception. Science, 322, 115–117. doi: 10.1126/science.1159845 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Whitson, J. A., Galinsky, A. D. & Kay, A. (2015). The emotional roots of conspiratorial perceptions, system justification, and belief in the paranormal. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 56, 89–95. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2014.09.002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wilson, M. S. & Rose, C. (2014). The role of paranoia in a dual-process motivational model of conspiracy belief. In J.-W. van ProoijenP. A. M. van LangeEds., Power, politics, and paranoia: Why people are suspicious of their leaders (pp. 273–291). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Yamaguchi, S., Kuhlman, D. M. & Sugimori, S. (1995). Personality correlates of allocentric tendencies in individualist and collectivist cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 26, 658–672. doi: 10.1177/002202219502600609 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Yzerbyt, V., Muller, D. & Judd, C. M. (2004). Adjusting researchers’ approach to adjustment: On the use of covariates when testing interactions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 424–431. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2003.10.001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar