Skip to main content
Original Article

Safety in Beauty

Social Exclusion, Antisocial Responses, and the Desire to Reconnect

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000311

Abstract. Individuals regularly exhibit antisocial responses after social exclusion. In four unregistered studies (1a, 1b, 2, and 3) and one preregistered experiment (Study 4), we tested the hypothesis that the excluder’s physical attractiveness reduces the relationship between social exclusion and negative responding. Results showed that exclusion by a highly attractive source caused less aggressive and more prosocial responses than exclusion by a less attractive source (Studies 1–3). The interaction effect was mediated by perceived likeability of the excluding person (Study 3). The preregistered experiment did not confirm the interactive effect between exclusion and attractiveness (Study 4); however, exploratory analyses indicated the effect on pro- (but not antisocial) responding. Inconsistent findings as well as the theoretical and practical implications are discussed.

References

  • Agthe, M., Spörrle, M., & Maner, J. K. (2011). Does being attractive always help? Positive and negative effects of attractiveness on social decision-making. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37, 1042–1054. doi: 10.1177/0146167211410355 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Aydin, N., Fischer, P., & Frey, D. (2010). The effects of social exclusion on religiousness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 742–753. doi: 10.1177/0146167210367491 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Aydin, N., Graupmann, V., Fischer, J., Frey, D., & Fischer, P. (2011). My role is my castle: The appeal of family roles after experiencing social exclusion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 981–986. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.03.020 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Aydin, N., Krueger, J., Fischer, J., Hahn, D., Frey, D., Kastenmüller, A., & Fischer, P. (2012). “Man’s best friend:” How the presence of a dog reduces mental distress after social exclusion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 446–449. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.09.011 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ayduk, Ö., Gyurak, A., & Luerssen, A. (2008). Individual differences in the rejection-aggression link in the hot sauce paradigm: The case of rejection sensitivity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 775–782. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2007.07.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bernstein, M. J., Young, S. G., Brown, C. M., Sacco, D. F., & Claypool, H. M. (2008). Adaptive responses to social exclusion: Social rejection improves detection of real and fake smiles. Psychological Science, 19, 981–983. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02187.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brunstein, J. C. (1993). Personal goals and subjective well-being: A longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 1061–1070. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.65.5.1061 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Buss, D. M. (1988). The evolution of human intrasexual competition: Tactics of mate attraction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 616–628. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.4.616 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12, 1–49. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00023992 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cacioppo, J. T., Hawkley, L. C., & Bernston, G. G. (2003). The anatomy of loneliness. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 71–74. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.01232 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chen, Z., & Williams, K. D. (2012). Imagined future social pain hurts more now than imagined future physical pain. European Journal of Social Psychology, 42, 314–317. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.186 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chester, D. S., Pond, R. S., Richman, S. B., & DeWall, C. N. (2012). The optimal calibration hypothesis: How life history modulates the brain’s social pain network. Frontiers in Evolutionary Neuroscience, 4, 10. doi: 10.3389/fnevo.2012.00010 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • DeWall, C. N., & Baumeister, R. F. (2006). Alone but feeling no pain: Effects of social exclusion on physical pain tolerance and pain threshold, affective forecasting, and interpersonal empathy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 1–15. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.1.1 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • DeWall, C. N., Maner, J. K., & Rouby, D. A. (2009). Social exclusion and early-stage interpersonal perception: Selective attention to signs of acceptance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 729–741. doi: 10.1037/a0014634 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • DeWall, C. N., & Richman, S. B. (2011). Social exclusion and the desire to reconnect. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 5, 919–932. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2011.00383.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • DeWall, C. N., Twenge, J. M., Bushman, B. J., Im, C., & Williams, K. D. (2010). Acceptance by one differs from acceptance by none: Applying social impact theory to the rejection-aggression link. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 1, 168–174. doi: 10.1177/1948550610361387 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dijkstra, P., & Buunk, B. P. (1998). Jealousy as a function of rival characteristics: An evolutionary perspective. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 1158–1166. doi: 10.1177/01461672982411003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dijkstra, J. K., Lindenberg, S., Verhulst, F. C., Ormel, J., & Veenstra, R. (2009). The relation between popularity and aggressive, destructive, and norm-breaking behaviors: Moderating effects of athletic abilities, physical attractiveness, and prosociality. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 19, 401–413. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2009.00594.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dion, K. K. (1972). Physical attractiveness and evaluation of children’s transgressions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24, 207–213. doi: 10.1037/h0033372 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dion, K., Bercheid, E., & Walster, E. (1972). What is beautiful is good. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24, 285–290. doi: 10.1037/h0033731 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Downey, G., & Feldman, S. I. (1996). Implications of rejection sensitivity for intimate relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 1327–1343. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.70.6.1327 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Downs, A. C., & Lyons, P. M. (1991). Natural observations of the links between attractiveness and initial legal judgments. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17, 541–547. doi: 10.1177/0146167291175009 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Eagly, A. H., Ashmore, R. D., Makhijani, M. G., & Longo, L. C. (1991). What is beautiful is good, but…: A meta-analytic review of research on the physical attractiveness stereotype. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 109–128. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.110.1.109 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Efran, M. G. (1974). The effect of physical appearance on the judgment of guilt, interpersonal attraction and severity of recommended punishment in a simulated jury task. Journal of Research in Personality, 8, 45–54. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.110.1.109 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., & Williams, K. D. (2003). Does rejection hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion. Science, 302, 290–292. doi: 10.1126/science.1089134 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Feingold, A. (1992). Good-looking people are not what we think. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 304–341. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.111.2.304 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Finkelstein, L. M., DeMuth, R. L. F., & Sweeney, D. L. (2007). Bias against overweight job applicants: Further explorations of when and why. Human Resource Management Journal, 46, 203–222. doi: 10.1002/hrm.20157 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fiske, S. T., & Yamamoto, M. (2005). Coping with rejection: Core social motives across cultures. In K. D. WilliamsJ. P. ForgasW. von HippelEds., The social outcast: Ostracism, social exclusion, rejection, and bullying (pp. 185–198). New York, NY: Psychology Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Gaertner, L., Iuzzini, J., & O’Mara, E. M. (2008). When rejection by one fosters aggression against many: Multiple-victim aggression as a consequence of social rejection and perceived groupness. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 958–970. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2008.02.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gardner, W. L., Pickett, C. L., & Brewer, M. B. (2000). Social exclusion and selective memory: How the need to belong influences memory for social events. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 486–496. doi: 10.1177/0146167200266007 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gerber, J., & Wheeler, L. (2009). On being rejected: A meta-analysis of experimental research on rejection. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 468–488. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01158.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Godwin, A., MacNevin, G., Zadro, L., Iannuzzelli, R., Weston, S., Gonsalkorale, K., & Devine, P. (2014). Are all ostracism experiences equal? A comparison of the autobiographical recall, Cyberball, and O-Cam paradigms. Behavior Research Methods, 46, 660–667. doi: 10.3758/s13428-013-0408-0 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gründl, M. (2013). Determinanten physischer Attraktivität – der Einfluss von Durchschnittlichkeit, Symmetrie und sexuellem Dimorphismus auf die Attraktivität von Gesichtern, [Determinants of physical attractiveness – the impact of averageness, symmetry and sexual dimorphism on human facial attractiveness]. Regensburg, Germany: Habilitation, University of Regensburg. Retrieved from http://www.beautycheck.de/cmsms/index.php/habilitationsschrift First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Haas, A., & Gregory, S. W. (2005). The impact of physical attractiveness on women’s social status and interactional power. Sociological Forum, 20, 449–471. doi: 10.1007/s11206-005-6597-2 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hitlan, R. T., Kelly, K. M., Schepman, S., Schneider, K. T., & Zárate, M. A. (2006). Language exclusion and the consequences of perceived ostracism in the workplace. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 10, 56–70. doi: 10.1037/1089-2699.10.1.56 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Iannetti, G. D., Salomons, T. V., Moayedi, M., Mouraux, A., & Davis, K. D. (2013). Beyond metaphor: contrasting mechanisms of social and physical pain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17, 371–378. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.06.002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kampe, K. W., Frith, C. D., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, U. (2001). Reward value of attractiveness and gaze: Making eye contact enhances the appeal of a pleasing face, irrespective of gender. Nature, 413, 589–590. doi: 10.1038/35098149 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Knowles, M. L., & Gardner, W. L. (2008). Benefits of membership: The activation and amplification of group identities in response to social rejection. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 1200–1213. doi: 10.1177/0146167208320062 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Krebs, D., & Adinolfi, A. A. (1975). Physical attractiveness, social relations, and personality style. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 245–253. doi: 10.1037/h0076286 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lakin, J. L., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). Using nonconscious behavioral mimicry to create affiliation and rapport. Psychological Science, 14, 334–339. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.14481 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Langlois, J. H., Kalakanis, L., Rubenstein, A. J., Larson, A., Hallam, M., & Smoot, M. (2000). Maxims or myths of beauty? A meta-analytic and theoretical review. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 390–423. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.3.390 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Leary, M. R., Kelly, K. M., Cottrell, C. A., & Schreindorfer, L. S. (2013). Individual differences in the need to belong: Mapping the nomological network. Journal of Personality Assessment, 95, 610–624. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2013.819511 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Leary, M. R., Kowalski, R. M., Smith, L., & Phillips, S. (2003). Teasing, rejection, and violence: Case studies of the school shootings. Aggressive Behavior, 29, 202–214. doi: 10.1002/ab.10061 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Leary, M. R., Tambor, E. S., Terdal, S. J., & Downs, D. L. (1995). Self-esteem as an interpersonal monitor. The sociometer hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 518–530. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.68.3.518 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lemay, E. P. Jr., Clark, M. S., & Greenberg, A. (2010). What is beautiful is good because what is beautiful is desired: Physical attractiveness stereotyping as projection of interpersonal goals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 339–353. doi: 10.1177/0146167209359700 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Luxen, M. F., & Van de Vijver, F. J. R. (2006). Facial attractiveness, sexual selection, and personnel selection: When evolved preferences matter. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 27, 241–255. doi: 10.1002/job.357 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • MacDonald, G., & Leary, M. R. (2005). Why does social exclusion hurt? The relationship between social and physical pain. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 202–223. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.2.202 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Maner, J. K., DeWall, C. N., Baumeister, R. F., & Schaller, M. (2007). Does social exclusion motivate interpersonal reconnection? Resolving the “porcupine problem”. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 42–55. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.92.1.42 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Maner, J. K., Gailliot, M. T., Rouby, D. A., & Miller, S. L. (2007). Can’t take my eyes off you: Attentional adhesion to mates and rivals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 389–401. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.93.3.389 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Maner, J. K., Miller, S. L., Schmidt, N. B., & Eckel, L. A. (2010). The endocrinology of exclusion: Rejection elicits motivationally tuned changes in progesterone. Psychological Science, 21, 581–588. doi: 10.1177/0956797610362676 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Molden, D. C., Lucas, G. M., Gardner, W. L., Dean, K., & Knowles, M. L. (2009). Motivations for prevention or promotion following social exclusion: Being rejected versus being ignored. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 415–431. doi: 10.1037/a0012958 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Molden, D. C., & Maner, J. K. (2013). How and when exclusion motivates social reconnection. In C. N. DeWallEd., The Oxford handbook of social exclusion (pp. 121–131). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Pfundmair, M., DeWall, C. N., Fries, V., Geiger, B., Krämer, T., Krug, S., … Aydin, N. (2015). Sugar or spice: Using I3 metatheory to understand how and why glucose reduces rejection-related aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 41, 537–543. doi: 10.1002/ab.21593 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pickett, C. L., Gardner, W. L., & Knowles, M. (2004). Getting a cue: The need to belong and enhanced sensitivity to social cues. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1095–1107. doi: 10.1177/0146167203262085 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879–891. doi: 10.3758/BRM.40.3.879 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Reijntjes, A., Thomaes, S., Kamphius, J. H., Bushmann, B. J., Orobio de Castro, B., & Telch, M. J. (2011). Explaining the paradoxical rejection-aggression link: The mediating effects of hostile intent attributions, anger, and decreases in state self-esteem on peer rejection-induced aggression in youth. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37, 955–963. doi: 10.1177/0146167211410247 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Richman, L., & Leary, M. R. (2009). Reactions to discrimination, stigmatization, ostracism, and other forms of interpersonal rejection: A multimotive model. Psychological Review, 116, 365–383. doi: 10.1037/a0015250 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Riva, P., Williams, K. D., Torstrick, A. M., & Montali, L. (2014). Orders to shoot (a camera): Effect of ostracism on obedience. Journal of Social Psychology, 154, 208–216. doi: 10.1080/00224545.2014.883354 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Romero-Canyas, R., Downey, G., Reddy, K. S., Rodriguez, S., Cavanaugh, T. J., & Pelayo, R. (2010). Paying to belong: When does rejection trigger ingratiation? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 802–823. doi: 10.1037/a0020013 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schoel, C., Eck, J., & Greifeneder, R. (2014). A matter of vertical position consequences of ostracism differ for those above versus below its perpetrators. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 5, 149–157. doi: 10.1177/1948550613488953 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Steffensmeier, D. J., & Terry, R. M. (1973). Deviance and respectability: An observational study of reactions to shoplifting. Social Forces, 51, 417–426. doi: 10.1093/sf/51.4.417 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Twenge, J. M., Baumeister, R. F., DeWall, N. C., Ciarocco, N. J., & Bartels, J. M. (2007). Social exclusion decreases prosocial behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 56–66. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.92.1.56 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Twenge, J. M., Baumeister, R. F., Tice, D. M., & Stucke, T. S. (2001). If you can’t join them, beat them: Effects of social exclusion on aggressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 1058–1069. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.81.6.1058 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Twenge, J. M., Zhang, L., Catanese, K. R., Dolan-Pascoe, B., Lyche, L. R., & Baumeister, R. F. (2007). Replenishing connectedness: Reminders of social activity reduce aggression after social exclusion. The British Journal of Social Psychology, 46, 205–224. doi: 10.1348/014466605X90793 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tuscherer, T., Sacco, D. F., Wirth, J. H., Claypool, H. M., Hugenberg, K., & Wesselmann, E. D. (2015). Responses to exclusion are moderated by its perceived fairness. European Journal of Social Psychology, 46, 280–293. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2152 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Warburton, W. A., Williams, K. D., & Cairns, D. R. (2006). When ostracism leads to aggression: The moderating effects of control deprivation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 213–220. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2152 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wesselmann, E. D., Bagg, D., & Williams, K. D. (2009). “I feel your pain”: The effects of observing ostracism on the ostracism detection system. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 1308–1311. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2009.08.003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Williams, K. D. (2007). Ostracism. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 425–452. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085641 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Williams, K. D. (2009). Ostracism: A temporal need-threat model. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 279–314. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(08)00406-1 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Williams, K. D., Cheung, C. K. T., & Choi, W. (2000). Cyberostracism: Effects of being ignored over the Internet. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 748–762. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.79.5.748 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Williams, K. D., & Jarvis, B. (2006). Cyberball: A program for use in research on ostracism and interpersonal acceptance. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 38, 174–180. doi: 10.3758/BF03192765 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Williams, K. D., & Sommer, K. L. (1997). Social ostracism by one’s coworkers: Does rejection lead to loafing or compensation? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 693–706. doi: 10.1177/0146167297237003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wirth, J. H., Sacco, D. F., Hugenberg, K., & Williams, K. D. (2010). Eye gaze as relational evaluation: Averted eye gaze leads to feelings of ostracism and relational devaluation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 869–882. doi: 10.1177/0146167210370032 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zadro, L., Kipling, K. D., & Richardson, R. (2004). How low can you go? Ostracism by a computer is sufficint to lower self-reported levels of belonging, control, self-esteem, and meaningful existence. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 560–567. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2003.11.006 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar