Skip to main content
Original Article

Taking Priming to Task

Variations in Stereotype Priming Effects Across Participant Task

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000326

Abstract. The current research examined potential moderators of gender and racial stereotype priming in sequential priming paradigms. Results from five experiments suggest that stereotype priming effects are more consistent in tasks that elicit both semantic priming and response competition (i.e., response priming paradigms) rather than tasks that evoke semantic priming alone (i.e., semantic priming paradigms). Recommendations for future stereotype priming research and the implication of these results for the proper interpretation of stereotype priming effects are discussed.

References

  • Adelman, J. S., Johnson, R. L., McCormick, S. F., McKague, M., Kinoshita, S., Bowers, J. S., … Davis, C. J. (2014). A behavioral database for masked form priming. Behavior Research Methods, 46, 1052–1067. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-013-0442-y First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Banaji, M. R. & Hardin, C. D. (1996). Automatic stereotyping. Psychological Science, 7, 136–141. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00346.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bartholow, B. D., Dickter, C. L. & Sestir, M. A. (2006). Stereotype activation and control of race bias: Cognitive control of inhibition and its impairment by alcohol. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 272–287. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.90.2.272 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bean, M. G., Stone, J., Moskowitz, G. B., Badger, T. A. & Focella, E. S. (2013). Evidence of nonconscious stereotyping of Hispanic patients by nursing and medical students. Nursing Research, 62, 362–367. https://doi.org/10.1097/NNR.0b013e31829e02ec First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bessenoff, G. R. & Sherman, J. W. (2000). Automatic and controlled components of prejudice toward fat people: Evaluation versus stereotype activation. Social Cognition, 18, 329–353. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2000.18.4.329 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Blair, I. V. (2002). The malleability of automatic stereotypes and prejudice. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6, 242. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327957PSPR0603_8 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Blair, I. V. & Banaji, M. R. (1996). Automatic and controlled processes in stereotype priming. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 1142. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.70.6.1142 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cacciari, C. & Padovani, R. (2007). Further evidence of gender stereotype priming in language: Semantic facilitation and inhibition in Italian role nouns. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 277–293. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716407070142 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carson, D. R. & Burton, A. M. (2001). Semantic priming of person recognition: Categorial priming may be a weaker form of the associative priming effect. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 54A, 1155–1179. https://doi.org/10.1080/02724980143000091 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Casper, C., Rothermund, K. & Wentura, D. (2010). Automatic stereotype activation is context dependent. Social Psychology, 41, 131–136. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000019 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Castelli, L., Macrae, C. N., Zogmaister, C. & Arcuri, L. (2004). A tale of two primes: Contextual limits on stereotype activation. Social Cognition, 22, 233–247. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.22.2.233.35462 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chwilla, D. J. & Kolk, H. J. (2005). Accessing world knowledge: Evidence from N400 and reaction time priming. Cognitive Brain Research, 25, 589–606. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.08.011 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Clow, K. A. & Esses, V. M. (2007). Expectancy effects in social stereotyping: Automatic and controlled processing in the Neely paradigm. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science/Revue Canadienne Des Sciences Du Comportement, 39, 161–173. https://doi.org/10.1037/cjbs20070013 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Collins, A. M. & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82, 407. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.82.6.407 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Conrey, F. R., Sherman, J. W., Gawronski, B., Hugenberg, K. & Groom, C. J. (2005). Separating multiple processes in implicit social cognition: The quad model of implicit task performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 469–487. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.89.4.469 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Deutsch, R. & Gawronski, B. (2009). When the method makes a difference: Antagonistic effects on “automatic evaluations” as a function of task characteristics of the measure. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 101–114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2008.09.001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Freng, S. & Willis-Esqueda, C. (2011). A question of honor: Chief Wahoo and American Indian stereotype activation among a university based sample. The Journal of Social Psychology, 151, 577–591. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2010.507265 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Habibi, R. & Khurana, B. (2012). Spontaneous gender categorization in masking and priming studies: Key for distinguishing Jane from John Doe but not Madonna from Sinatra. PloS One, 7, 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0032377 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hehman, E., Volpert, H. I. & Simons, R. F. (2014). The N400 as an index of racial stereotype accessibility. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9, 544–552. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nst018 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Higgins, E. T. & King, G. (1981). Accessibility of social constructs: Information processing consequences of individual and contextual variability. Personality, Cognition, and Social Interaction, 69, 121. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hutchison, K. A. (2003). Is semantic priming due to association strength or feature overlap? A microanalytic review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 785–813. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196544 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Karylowski, J. J., Motes, M. A., Wallace, H. M., Harckom, H. A., Hewlett, E. M., Maclean, S. L., … Vaswani, C. L. (2001). Spontaneous gender-stereotypical categorization of trait labels and job labels. Current Research in Social Psychology, 6, 77–90. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kawakami, K., Dion, K. L. & Dovidio, J. F. (1999). Implicit stereotyping and prejudice and the primed Stroop task. Swiss Journal of Psychology/Schweizerische Zeitschrift Für Psychologie/Revue Suisse De Psychologie, 58, 241–250. https://doi.org/10.1024/1421-0185.58.4.241 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Kawakami, K. & Dovidio, J. F. (2001). The reliability of implicit stereotyping. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 212–225. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167201272007 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kawakami, K., Dovidio, J. F., Moll, J., Hermsen, S. & Russin, A. (2000). Just say no (to stereotyping): Effects of training in the negation of stereotypic associations on stereotype activation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 871–888. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.78.5.871 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kidder, C., White, K., Hinojos, M., Sandoval, M. & Crites, S. L. (2017). Sequential stereotype priming: A meta-analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Review, Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868317723532 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kimura, A., Wada, Y., Goto, S., Tsuzuki, D., Cai, D., Oka, T. & Dan, I. (2009). Implicit gender-based food stereotypes. Semantic priming experiments on young Japanese. Appetite, 52, 521–524. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2008.11.002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lucas, M. (2000). Semantic priming without association: A meta-analytic review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7, 618–630. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03212999 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Macrae, C. N. & Cloutier, J. (2009). A matter of design: Priming context and person perception. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 1012–1015. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2009.04.021 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Macrae, C. N., Hood, B. M., Milne, A. B., Rowe, A. C. & Mason, M. F. (2002). Are you looking at me? Eye gaze and person perception. Psychological Science, 13, 460–464. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00481 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Macrae, C. N. & Martin, D. (2007). A boy primed Sue: Feature-based processing and person construal. European Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 793–805. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.406 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Macrae, C. N., Mitchell, J. P. & Pendry, L. F. (2002). What’s in a forename? Cue familiarity and stereotypical thinking. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 186–193. https://doi.org/10.1006/jesp.2001.1496 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Masson, M. J. (1995). A distributed memory model of semantic priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 3–23. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.21.1.3 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McNamara, T. P. (2005). Semantic priming: Perspectives from memory and word recognition. New York, NY: Psychology Press. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203338001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Minear, M. & Park, D. C. (2004). A lifespan database of adult facial stimuli. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 36, 630–633. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03206543 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Moskowitz, G. B. & Li, P. (2011). Egalitarian goals trigger stereotype inhibition: A proactive form of stereotype control. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 103–116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp. 2010.08.014 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Moss, H. E., Ostrin, R. K., Tyler, L. K. & Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1995). Accessing different types of lexical semantic information: Evidence from priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 863–883. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.21.4.863 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Müller, F. & Rothermund, K. (2014). What does it take to activate stereotypes? Simple primes don’t seem to be enough. A replication of stereotype activation (Banaji & Hardin, 1996; Blair & Banaji, 1996). Social Psychology, 45, 187–193. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000183 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Neely, J. H. (1977). Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited-capacity attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 106, 226–254. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.106.3.226 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Neely, J. H., Keefe, D. E. & Ross, K. L. (1989). Semantic priming in the lexical decision task: Roles of prospective prime-generated expectancies and retrospective semantic matching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15(6), 1003–1019. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.15.6.1003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Plaza, M., Boiché, J., Brunel, L. & Ruchaud, F. (2017). Sport = Male… But not all sports: Investigating the gender stereotypes of sport activities at the explicit and implicit levels. Sex Roles, 76, 202–217. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-016-0650-x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Postman, L. & Keppel, G. (1970). Norms of word association. New York, NY: Academic Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Rastle, K., Harrington, J. & Coltheart, M. (2002). 358,534: The ARC Nonword Database. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55A, 1339–1362. https://doi.org/10.1080/02724980244000099 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Righi, G., Peissig, J. J. & Tarr, M. J. (2012). Recognizing disguised faces. Visual Cognition, 20, 143–169. https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2012.654624 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rohmer, O. & Louvet, E. (2012). Implicit measures of the stereotype content associated with disability. British Journal of Social Psychology, 51, 732–740. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.2011.02087.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rumelhart, D. E. & McClelland, J. L. (1986). The PDP Research Group 1986: Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition (Vols. 1 and 2). Cambridge, MA: Foundations, MIT Press/Bradford Books. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Sassenberg, K. & Moskowitz, G. B. (2005). Don’t stereotype, think different! Overcoming automatic stereotype activation by mindset priming. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 506–514. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2004.10.002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schacter, D. L., Dobbins, I. G. & Schnyer, D. M. (2004). Specificity of priming: A cognitive neuroscience perspective. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 853–862. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1534 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schacter, D. L., Gutchess, A. H. & Kensinger, E. A. (2009). Specificity of memory: Implications for individual and collective remembering. In P. BoyerJ. V. WertschP. BoyerJ. V. WertschEds., Memory in mind and culture (pp. 83–111). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511626999.006 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Spruyt, A., De Houwer, J., Everaert, T. & Hermans, D. (2012). Unconscious semantic activation depends on feature-specific attention allocation. Cognition, 122, 91–95. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.08.017 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Spruyt, A., De Houwer, J. & Hermans, D. (2009). Modulation of automatic semantic priming by feature-specific attention allocation. Journal of Memory and Language, 61, 37–54. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2009.03.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Spruyt, A., De Houwer, J., Hermans, D. & Eelen, P. (2007). Affective priming of nonaffective semantic categorization responses. Experimental Psychology (formerly “Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie”), 54, 44–53. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169.54.1.44 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Stewart, T. L., Latu, I. M., Kawakami, K. & Myers, A. C. (2010). Consider the situation: Reducing automatic stereotyping through situational attribution training. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 221–225. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2009.09.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Verhaeghen, P., Aikman, S. N. & Van Gulick, A. E. (2011). Prime and prejudice: Co-occurrence in the culture as a source of automatic stereotype priming. British Journal of Social Psychology, 50, 501–518. https://doi.org/10.1348/014466610X524254 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Voss, A., Rothermund, K., Gast, A. & Wentura, D. (2013). Cognitive processes in associative and categorical priming: A diffusion model analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 536–559. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029459 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wentura, D. & Degner, J. (2010). A practical guide to sequential priming and related tasks. In B. GawronskiB. K. PayneEds., Handbook of implicit social cognition: Measurement, theory, and applications (pp. 95–116). New York, NY: Guilford Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Wentura, D. & Rothermund, K. (2014). Priming is not priming is not priming. In D. C. MoldenD. C. MoldenEds., Understanding priming effects in social psychology (pp. 49–69). New York, NY: Guilford Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • White, K. R. G. & Crites, S. L. (2009). Gender stereotype priming in RTs and the N400. Unpublished raw data. El Paso, TX: University of Texas at El Paso. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • White, K. R., Crites, S. L., Taylor, J. H. & Corral, G. (2009). Wait, what? Assessing stereotype incongruities using the N400 ERP component. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 4, 191–198. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsp004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wiese, H. & Schweinberger, S. R. (2008). Event-related potentials indicate different processes to mediate categorical and associative priming in person recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 1246–1263. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012937 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wittenbrink, B. (2007). Measuring attitudes through priming. In B. WittenbrinkN. SchwarzEds., Implicit measures of attitudes (pp. 17–58). New York, NY: Guilford Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Wittenbrink, B., Judd, C. M. & Park, B. (1997). Evidence for racial prejudice at the implicit level and its relationship with questionnaire measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 262. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.72.2.262 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar