Skip to main content
Original Article

Proportional Reasoning

The Role of Congruity and Salience in Behavioral and Imaging Research

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/a000262

Abstract. Comparison of ratios is difficult for children and adults. We studied the role of salience and congruity in comparison of ratios using reaction time and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Participants were asked to decide which of two mixtures of red and white paint drops (presented in Arabic numerals) was darker. In congruent trials the mixture with more red drops was darker and in incongruent trials it was lighter. Half of the trials were red salience (more red than white drops in both mixtures) and half of them were white salience. Interaction between congruity and salience was observed. Behaviorally, accuracy was higher and reaction time of correct responses (RTC) was shorter in congruent red salience and incongruent white salience conditions. For these conditions higher activation in a fronto-parietal numerical network was observed in fMRI. These findings suggest that automatic processing of natural numbers supports or suppresses the comparison of ratios as a function of congruity and salience.

References

  • Arsalidou, M. & Taylor, M. J. (2011). Is 2 + 2=4? Meta-analyses of brain areas needed for numbers and calculations. NeuroImage, 54, 2382–2393. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.009 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Babai, R., Brecher, T., Stavy, R. & Tirosh, D. (2006). Intuitive interference in probabilistic reasoning. International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education, 4, 627–639. doi: 10.1007/s10763-006-9031-1 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Babai, R., Nattiv, L. & Stavy, R. (2016). Comparison of perimeters: Improving students’ performance by increasing the salience of the relevant variable. ZDM Mathematics Education, 48, 367–378. doi: 10.1007/s11858-016-0766-z First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bonato, M., Fabbri, S., Umilta, C. & Zorzi, M. (2007). The mental representation of numerical fractions: Real or integer? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 1410–1419. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.33.6.1410 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cox, R. W. (1996). AFNI: Software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Computers and Biomedical Research, 29, 162–173. doi: 10.1006/cbmr.1996.0014 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dehaene, S. (2011). The number sense: How the mind creates mathematics. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Dehaene, S. & Cohen, L. (1995). Towards an anatomical and functional model of number processing. Mathematical Cognition, 1, 82–120. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Dehaene, S. & Cohen, L. (1997). Cerebral pathways for calculation: Double dissociation between rote verbal and quantitative knowledge of arithmetic. Cortex, 33, 219–250. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70002-9 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fischbein, E. (1987). Intuition in science and mathematics. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Reidel. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Fischer, M. H., Castel, A. D., Dodd, M. D. & Pratt, J. (2003). Perceiving numbers causes spatial shifts of attention. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 555–556. doi: 10.1038/nn1066 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Green, D. R. (1983). A survey of probabilistic concepts in 3000 students aged 11–16 years. In D. R. GreyP. HolmesV. BarnettG. M. ConstableEds., Proceedings of the First International Conference on Teaching Statistics (pp. 766–783). Sheffield, UK: Teaching Statistics Trust. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Inhelder, B. & Piaget, J. (1958). The growth of logical thinking: From childhood to adolescence. New York, NY: Basic Books. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ischebeck, A., Schocke, M. & Delazer, M. (2009). The processing and representation of fractions within the brain: An fMRI investigation. NeuroImage, 47, 403–413. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.03.041 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jacob, S. N. & Nieder, A. (2009). Notation-independent representation of fractions in the human parietal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 4652–4657. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0651-09.2009 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kallai, A. Y. & Tzelgov, J. (2009). A generalized fraction: An entity smaller than one on the mental number line. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 1845–1864. doi: 10.1037/a0016892 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Karplus, E., Pulos, S. & Stage, E. K. (1983). Proportional reasoning of early adolescents. In R. LeshM. LandauEds., Acquisition of mathematics concepts and processes (pp. 45–90). New York, NY: Academic Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Lamon, S. J. (2007). Rational numbers and proportional reasoning: Toward a theoretical framework for research. In F. LesterEd., Second handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 629–668). Reston, VA: NCTM. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Meert, G., Grégoire, J. & Noël, M.-P. (2009). Rational numbers: Componential versus holistic representation of fractions in a magnitude comparison task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 1598–1616. doi: 10.1080/17470210802511162 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Moss, J. & Case, R. (1999). Developing children’s understanding of the rational numbers: A new model and an experimental curriculum. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 30, 122–147. doi: 10.2307/749607 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Noelting, G. (1980a). The development of proportional reasoning and the ratio concept (the Orange Juice Experiment). Part 1: The determination of stages. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 11, 217–253. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Noelting, G. (1980b). The development of proportional reasoning and the ratio concept. Part 2: Problem structure at successive stages; problem-solving strategies and the mechanism of adaptive restructuring. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 11, 331–363. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Olk, B. (2013). Measuring the allocation of attention in the Stroop task: Evidence from eye movement patterns. Psychological Research, 77, 106–115. doi: 10.1007/s00426-011-0405-9 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Osman, M. & Stavy, R. (2006). Development of intuitive rules: Evaluating the application of the dual-system framework to understanding children’s intuitive reasoning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 935–953. doi: 10.3758/BF03213907 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Reiss, M., Behr, M., Lesh, R. & Post, T. (1985). Cognitive processes and products in proportional reasoning. In L. StreeflandEd., Proceedings of the Ninth International Conference for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (pp. 352–356). Noordwijkerhout (Utrecht), The Netherlands: PME. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Siegler, R. S. (1976). Three aspects of cognitive development. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 481–520. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stavy, R. & Babai, R. (2008). Complexity of shapes and quantitative reasoning in geometry. Mind, Brain, and Education, 2, 170–176. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-228X.2008.00051.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stavy, R. & Babai, R. (2010). Overcoming intuitive interference in mathematics: Insights from behavioral, brain imaging and intervention studies. ZDM Mathematics Education, 42, 621–633. doi: 10.1007/s11858-010-0251-z First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stavy, R., Goel, V., Critchley, H. & Dolan, R. (2006). Intuitive interference in quantitative reasoning. Brain Research, 1073–1074, 383–388. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.12.011 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stavy, R. & Tirosh, D. (2000). How students (mis-)understand science and mathematics. New York, NY: Teachers College Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar