Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1024/2235-0977/a000025

Zusammenfassung: Schlafstörungen im Kindesalter sind weit verbreitet. Häufig wirken sich kindliche Schlafprobleme auf die kognitive Leistungsfähigkeit und damit auf die schulischen Leistungen aus. Ziel der vorliegenden Arbeit war es, eine Übersicht über den Zusammenhang zwischen den Schlafparametern Nachtschlaf, Schlafdauer, Schlafeffizienz, Schlafqualität, Schlafprobleme sowie der kognitiven und schulischen Leistungsfähigkeit im Kindesalter (fünf bis 13 Jahre) zu geben. Einige der 25 gefundenen Studien konnten aufzeigen, dass der Nachtschlaf den Kindern das Lernen erleichtert. Dabei konnten Aufgaben, die keine tiefen Verarbeitungsprozesse benötigten, von Kindern mit Schlafeinbußen zumeist kompensiert werden, Aufgaben mit tieferen Verarbeitungsprozessen hingegen häufig nicht. Der Nachtschlaf scheint sich in diesem Alter vor allem auf die deklarative Gedächtnisleistung positiv auszuwirken. Demnach scheint die Gedächtniskonsolidierung während des Schlafs vom Typ des Lernmaterials abzuhängen. Eine zu kurze Schlafdauer, zu geringe Schlafeffizienz und das Vorhandensein von Schlafproblemen hängen überwiegend mit Einbußen bei kognitiven Lernprozessen im Kindesalter zusammen. Hingegen korrelierten subjektive Schlafqualität und Lernen nicht miteinander. Insgesamt existieren noch wenige Studien, die den Zusammenhang zwischen den verschiedenen Schlafparametern und kognitiven Prozessen der Kinder untersuchen. Weitere systematische Studien sind daher sinnvoll und notwendig, die überdies beeinflussende Variablen, wie zum Beispiel elterliche Kontrolle, in die Betrachtung einbeziehen. Da Schlafstörungen bei Kindern eine Gefahr für die kognitive und schulische Entwicklung darstellen, sollten sie frühzeitig diagnostiziert werden. Daher sollten Eltern, Lehrer und alle in der Erziehung tätigen Praktiker über ein ausreichendes Wissen zu den Symptomen und Folgen schlechten Schlafs bei Kindern sowie über geeignete Präventions- und Interventionsmöglichkeiten verfügen. Gezielte Förderprogramme zur Verbesserung der schulischen und kognitiven Leistungen der Kinder könnten eingesetzt werden.


Extended abstract

The Importance of Sleep and Sleep Disorders for Learning and Memory in Children – a Review

Background: Sleep plays a decisive role for learning, memory processing, and cognitive functioning. However, sleep can be disturbed in various ways. Hence sleep disorders in childhood are common. The prevalence rates vary between 20 % and 45 % (Esser & Schmidt, 1987; Hoedlmoser, Kloesch, Wiater & Schabus, 2010; Kahn et al., 1989; Mindell, 1993). Usually, children have difficulties falling asleep or maintaining sleep, show bed-time resistance, complain about poor quality of sleep or daytime sleepiness, or have parasomnia, e. g., nightmares (Hoedlmoser et al., 2010; Jenni, Fuhrer, Iglowstein, Molinari & Largo, 2005; Mindell & Barrett, 2002). Furthermore, sleep disorders in childhood often even persist (Schlarb, Velten-Schurian, Poets & Hautzinger, 2011) until adulthood (Johnson, Roth, Schultz & Breslau, 2006). Various variables (e. g., physiological maturing, temperament, behavior, individual circadian differences, physical factors, mental retardation, and stress) influence the development, maintenance, degree, and chronicity of sleep disorders in childhood (Jenni et al., 2005; Kraenz et al., 2004). In addition, parenting style, parental educational background, family stress as well as quality and quantity of parental sleep may affect the sleep behavior of children (Meltzer & Mindel, 2007; Schlarb et al., 2011). Hence, consequences of sleep problems in childhood are broad, and comprise cognitive, motivational, social, neurological, and cardiovascular restraints. Almost a fifth of children with sleep disorders report daytime sleepiness or limited attention and motivation (Blunden, Lushington, Lorenzen, Martin & Kennedy, 2005; Dahl, 1996; Kahn et al., 1989; Smedje, Broman & Hetta, 2001). Moreover, behavioral problems, e. g., social conflicts or aggressive behavior, are related to shortened sleep duration (Aronen, Paavonen, Fjällberg, Soininen & Törrönen, 2000; Gregory & O’Connor, 2002; Hoedlmoser et al., 2010; Paavonen, Porkka-Heiskanen & Lahikainen, 2009; Velten-Schurian, Hautzinger, Poets & Schlarb, 2010). Untreated persisting sleep problems can lead to serious and harmful consequences, e. g., neurological dysfunction (Jan et al., 2010), and impairment of the cardiovascular or immune system (Mullington, Haack, Toth, Serrador & Meier-Ewert, 2009). Shortened sleep duration and poor sleep quality may also lead to reduced cognitive abilities (Sadeh, Gruber & Raviv, 2002). Thus, children’s sleep difficulties are frequently associated with lower cognitive capabilities and lower school performance. There are diverse theories explaining the effect of inadequate sleep on cognitive functioning and behavior. First, sleep plays a major role in the maturation of the brain, information processing, memory consolidation, and learning (e. g., Born & Wilhelm, 2011; Wilhelm, Prehn-Kristensen & Born, 2012). Thus, inadequate sleep may affect all these processes and lead to problems with consolidation at night and retrieval of knowledge (Dewald, Meijer, Oort, Kerkhof & Bogels, 2010). Second, sleep serves as a regeneration process. Therefore, inadequate sleep may lead to daytime sleepiness and reduced attention. Moreover, cognitive functioning and regulation of behavior could be impaired due to restricted functionality of specific brain areas (Sadeh, 2007).

Aim: The aim of this paper was to provide an overview of the relationship between children’s sleep and sleep disorders, on the one hand, and learning and cognitive or learning disabilities, on the other hand. We focused on various sleep parameters and cognitive and academic performance. Inclusion criteria were studies (a) of healthy children aged five to 13 years with or without difficulties initiating or maintaining sleep; (b) sleep parameters, i. e., sleep at night, sleep duration, sleep efficiency, sleep quality, sleep problems, assessed by self-report, parents, sleep reports, or objective instruments such as actigraphy; (c) cognitive or academic functioning, which were measured by standardized performance tests, memory tests, or reaction time tests.

Method: A literature research by DIMDI, MEDLINE, PsycINFO, PsycNET, PubMed, and Google Scholar based on the keywords ’sleep’, ’sleep disorder’, ’learning’, ’memory’, ’performance’ and ’children’ was conducted.

Results: Twenty-five studies were included, whose results were grouped by sleep parameters and are represented below: (a) Sleep at night seems to be an important factor for processing declarative knowledge, whereas the results for processing procedural or implicit knowledge in children are diverse. These results differ remarkably from the results of adults. (b) The results about the relationship between sleep duration and cognitive performance in children demonstrate that longer sleep duration appears to improve cognitive performance, while shortened sleep duration seems to restrain cognitive performance, particularly for tasks with a higher cognitive load. This effect is very likely to last for years. On the other hand, some authors found no relation between sleep duration and cognitive performance, particularly for tasks with a lower processing load. Brief sleep restrictions also had no effect on cognitive functioning in this age group. (c) Low sleep efficacy seems to impair cognitive performance in children. However, no connection between sleep efficacy and type or complexity of learning material was found. (d) Only one of the included studies investigated the relationship between subjective sleep quality and learning in childhood; it showed no correlation between sleep quality and the accuracy or speed of concentration capability, as measured by simple tasks involving continuous selective attention. (e) Additionally, studies showed that sleep problems impair the cognitive and academic performance of children, even for years.

Discussion: This overview shows that sleep appears to play a considerable role for cognitive processing and school performance. Sleep at night seems to facilitate declarative memory processing. Thus, memory consolidation during sleep may depend on the type of learning material. These results are in line with the neurocognitive hypothesis, which implies that sleep affects the neuronal processing as demonstrated by fMRI and EEG studies (Hoedlmoser et al., 2008; Schabus et al., 2006; Walker & Stickgold, 2006). Most studies showed a connection between sleep duration, sleep efficiency, sleep problems, and cognitive learning processes. To conclude, further studies addressing the relationship between various sleep parameters and cognitive processes in childhood should be conducted. Additional systematic studies with children should be performed taking into account biasing variables, e. g., parental control (Kopasz et al., 2010; Owens-Stively et al., 1997), and further factors, e. g., odor as a learning association, which may facilitate memory consolidation while sleeping (Rasch, Buchel, Gais & Born, 2007).

Implications: Sleep disorders in childhood endanger cognitive and school development, and should therefore be diagnosed early on. Hence, parents, teachers, and educational practitioners should have sufficient knowledge about the symptoms and consequences of poor sleep in children as well as about adequate prevention and intervention possibilities. Specific supporting programmes could be implemented to improve cognitive and academic functioning in children.

Literatur

  • Anders, T. F. , Eiben, L. A. (1997). Pediatric sleep disorders: a review of the past 10 years. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 9– 20. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Anderson, J. R. (1976). Language, memory, and thought.. Photomac, MD: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Aronen, E. T. , Paavonen, E. J. , Fjällberg, M. , Soininen, M. , Törrönen, J. (2000). Sleep and psychiatric symptoms in school-age children. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 502– 508. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Astill, R. G. , Van der Heijden, K. B. , Van Ijzendoorn, M. H. , Van Someren, E. J. (2012). Sleep, cognition, and behavioral problems in school-age children: A century of research meta-analyzed. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 1109– 1138. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Atkinson, R. C. , Shiffrin, R. M. (1968) Human memory: A proposed system and its control mechanisms. In K. W. Spence (Hrsg.), The psychology of learning and motivation: (2), 89– 195, New York: Academic Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Backhaus, J. , Hoeckesfeld, R. , Born, J. , Hohagen, F. , Junghanns, K. (2008). Immediate as well as delayed post learning sleep but not wakefulness enhances declarative memory consolidation in children. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 89, 76– 80. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Beebe, D. W. (2011). Cognitive, behavioral, and functional consequences of inadequate sleep in children and adolescents. Pediatric Clinics of North America, 58, 649– 665. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Biggs, S. N. , Bauer, K. M. M. , Peters, J. , Dorrian, J. , Kennedy, J. D. , Martin, A. J. et al. (2010). Acute sleep restriction does not affect declarative memory in 10-year-old girls. Sleep and Biological Rhythms, 8, 222– 225. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Birbaumer, N. , Schmidt, R. F. (2006). Lernen und Gedächtnis. In Schmidt, F. & Schaible, H.-G. (Hrsg.), . Neuro- und Sinnesphysiologie, S. 402– 424, Heidelberg: Springer. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Blunden, S. , Lushington, K. , Lorenzen, B. , Martin, J. , Kennedy, D. (2005). Neuropsychological and psychosocial function in children with a history of snoring or behavioral sleep problems. The Journal of Pediatrics, 146, 780– 6. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Born, J. , Rasch, B. , Gais, S. (2006). Sleep to remember. Neuroscientist, 12, 410– 424. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Born, J. , Wilhelm, I. (2011). System consolidation of memory during sleep. Psychological Research, 76, 192– 203. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bruni, O. , Verrillo, E. , Milano, S. , Ottaviano, S. (2000). Clinical an historical predictors of sleep disturbances in school-age children. Sleep and Hypnosis, 4, 147– 151. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bub, K. L. , Buckhalt, J. A. , El-Sheikh, M. (2011). Children's sleep and cognitive performance: a cross-domain analysis of change over time. Developmental Psychology, 47, 1504– 1514. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Buckhalt, J. A. (2011a). Insufficient Sleep and the Socioeconomic Status Achievement Gap. Child Development Perspectives, 5, 59– 65. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Buckhalt, J. A. (2011b). Children's sleep, sleepiness, and performance on cognitive tasks. WMF Press Bulletin, No. 2. Online publication on the Woodcock-Muñoz Foundation website:: http://woodcock-munoz-foundation.org/press/pressbulletins.html First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Buckhalt, J. A. , El-Sheikh, M. , Keller, P. S. , Kelly, R. J. (2009). Concurrent and longitudinal relations between children's sleep and cognitive functioning: the moderating role of parent education. Child Development, 80, 875– 982. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Buckhalt, J. A. , El-Sheikh, M. , Keller, P. (2007). Children's sleep and cognitive functioning: race and socioeconomic status as moderators of effects. Child Development, 78, 213– 231. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cohen, N. J. , Squire, L. R. (1980). Preserved lerning and retention of pattern-analyzing skill in amnesia: Dissociation of knowing how and knowing that. Science, 210, 207– 210. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cooper, P. , Kohler, M. , Blunden, S. (2012). Sleep and academic performance in Indigenous Australian children from a remote community: an exploratory study. Journal of Paediatrics and Child Health, 48, 122– 127. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Curcio, G. , Ferrara, M. , De Gennaro, L. (2006). Sleep loss, learning capacity and academic performance. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 10, 323– 337. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dahl, R. E. (1996). The impact of inadequate sleep on children's daytime cognitive function. Seminars in Pediatric Neurology, 3, 44– 50. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dewald, J. F. , Meijer, A. M. , Oort, F. J. , Kerkhof, G. A. , Bogels, S. M. (2010). The influence of sleep quality, sleep duration and sleepiness on school performance in children and adolescents: A meta-analytic review. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 14, 179– 189. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Diekelmann, S. , Wilhelm, I. , Born, J. (2009). The whats and whens of sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 13, 309– 321. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Edelmann, W. (2000). Lernpsychologie. Weinheim: Beltz PVU. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • El-Sheikh, M. , Buckhalt, J. A. , Keller, P. S. , Cummings, E. M. , Acebo, C. (2007). Child emotional insecurity and academic achievement: the role of sleep disruptions. Journal of Family Psychology, 21, 29– 38. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Esser, G. , Schmidt, M. H. (1987). Epidemiologie und Verlauf kinderpsychiatrischer Störungen im Schulalter – Ergebnisse einer Längsschnittstudie. Nervenheilkunde, 6, 27– 35. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Fallone, G. , Acebo, C. , Arnedt, J. T. , Seifer, R. , Carskadon, M. A. (2001). Effects of acute sleep restriction on behavior, sustained attention, and response inhibition in children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 93, 213– 229. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fischer, S. , Wilhelm, I. , Born, J. (2007). Developmental differences in sleep's role for implicit off-line learning: comparing children with adults. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 214– 227. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Friedman, N. P. , Corley, R. P. , Hewitt, J. K. , Wright, K. P., Jr. (2009). Individual differences in childhood sleep problems predict later cognitive executive control. Sleep, 32, 323– 333. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Geiger, A. , Achermann, P. , Jenni, O. G. (2010). Association be­tween sleep duration and intelligence scores in healthy children. Developmental Psychology, 46, 949– 954. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gomez, R. L. , Newman-Smith, K. C. , Breslin, J. H. , Bootzin, R. R. (2011). Learning, memory, and sleep in children. Sleep Medicine Clinics, 6, 45– 57. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gregory, A. M. , Caspi, A. , Moffitt, T. E. , Poulton, R. (2009). Sleep problems in childhood predict neuropsychological functioning in adolescence. Pediatrics, 123, 1171– 1176. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gregory, A. M. , O'Connor, T. G. (2002). Sleep problems in childhood: a longitudinal study of developmental change and association with behavioral problems. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 964– 71. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gruber, R. , Laviolette, R. , Deluca, P. , Monson, E. , Cornish, K. , Carrier, J. (2010). Short sleep duration is associated with poor performance on IQ measures in healthy school-age children. Sleep Medicine, 11, 289– 294. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Henderson, L. M. , Weighall, A. R. , Brown, H. , Gareth Gaskell, M. (2012). Consolidation of vocabulary is associated with sleep in children. Developmental Science, 15, 674– 687. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hoedlmoser, K. , Kloesch, G. , Wiater, A. , Schabus, M. (2010). Self-reported sleep patterns, sleep problems and behavioural problems among Austrian school children aged 8 – 11 years. Somnologie, 14, 23– 31. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hoedlmoser, K. , Pecherstorfer, T. , Gruber, G. , Anderer, P. , Doppelmayr, M. , Klimesch, W. et al. (2008). Instrumental conditioning of human sensorimotor rhythm (12 – 15 Hz) and its impact on sleep as well as declarative learning. Sleep, 31, 1401– 1408. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hosenfeld, I. , Schrader, F. W. (2006). Schulische Leistung: Grundlagen, Bedingungen, Perspektiven.. Münster: Waxmann. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Huber, R. , Ghilardi, M. F. , Massimini, M. , Tononi, G. (2004). Local sleep and learning. Nature, 430, 78– 81. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Iglowstein, I. , Jenni, O. G. , Molinari, L. , Largo, R. H. (2003). Sleep duration from infancy to adolescence: reference values and generational trends. Pediatrics, 111, 302– 307. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jan, J. E. , Reiter, R. J. , Bax, M. C. , Ribary, U. , Freeman, R. D. , Wasdell, M. B. (2010). Long-term sleep disturbances in children: a cause of neuronal loss. European Journal of Paediatric Neurology, 14, 380– 390. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jenni, O. G. , Fuhrer, H. Z. , Iglowstein, I. , Molinari, L. , Largo, R. H. (2005). A longitudinal study of bed sharing and sleep problems among Swiss children in the first 10 years of life. Pediatrics, 115, 223– 40. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Johnson, E. O. , Roth, T. , Schultz, L. , Breslau, N. (2006). ­Epidemiology of DSM-IV Insomnia in Adolescence: Lifetime Prevalence, Chronicity, and an Emergent Gender Difference. Pediatrics, 117, e247– e256. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kahn, A. , van de Merckt, C. , Rebuffat, E. , Mozin, M. J. , Sottiaux, M. , Blum, D. et al. (1989). Sleep problems in healthy preadolescents. Pediatrics, 84, 542– 546. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kopasz, M. , Loessl, B. , Hornyak, M. , Riemann, D. , Nissen, C. , Piosczyk, H. et al. (2010). Sleep and memory in healthy children and adolescents – a critical review. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 14, 167– 177. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kraenz, S. , Fricke, L. , Wiater, A. , Mitschke, A. , Breuer, U. , Lehmkuhl, G. (2004). Häufigkeit und Belastungsfaktoren bei Schlafstörungen im Einschulalter. Praxis der Kinderpsychologie und Kinderpsychiatrie, 53, 3– 18. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Lim, J. , Dinges, D. F. (2010). A meta-analysis of the impact of short-term sleep deprivation on cognitive variables. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 375– 389. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Meijer, A. M. , Habekothé, R. T. , van den Wittenboer, G. L. H. (2000). Time in bed, quality of sleep and school functioning of children. Journal of Sleep Research, 9, 145– 153. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Meltzer, L. J. , Mindell, J. A. (2007). Relationship between Child Sleep Disturbances and Maternal Sleep, Mood, and Parenting Stress: a Pilot Study. Journal of Family Psychology, 21, 67– 73. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mindell, J. A. , Barrett, K. M. (2002). Nightmares and anxiety in elementary aged children: is there a relationship?. Child: Care, Health and Development, 28, 317– 322. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mindell, J. A. (1993). Sleep disorders in children. Health Psychology, 12, 151– 162. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mullington, J. M. , Haack, M. , Toth, M. , Serrador, J. M. , Meier-Ewert, H. K. (2009). Cardiovascular, inflammatory, and metabolic consequences of sleep deprivation. Progress in Cardiovascular Diseases, 51, 294– 302. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Oberauer, K. , Mayr, U., Kluwe, R. (2006). Gedächtnis und Wissen.. In Spada, H. (Hrsg). Lehrbuch Allgemeine Psychologie, S. 115 – 195. Bern: Huber. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ohayon, M. M. , Carskadon, M. A. , Guilleminault, C. , Vitiello, M. V. (2004). Meta-analysis of quantitative sleep parameters from childhood to old age in healthy individuals: developing normative sleep values across the human lifespan. Sleep, 27, 1255– 1273. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Owens-Stively, J. , Frank, N. , Smith, A. , Hagino, O. , Spirito, A. , Arrigan, M. et al. (1997). Child temperament, parenting discipline style, and daytime behavior in childhood sleep disorders. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 18, 314– 321. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Paavonen, E. J. , Raikkonen, K. , Pesonen, A. K. , Lahti, J. , ­Komsi, N. , Heinonen, K. et al. (2010). Sleep quality and cognitive performance in 8-year-old children. Sleep Medicine, 11, 386– 392. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Paavonen, E. J. , Porkka-Heiskanen, T. , Lahikainen, A. R. (2009). Sleep quality, duration and behavioral symptoms among 5 – 6-year-old children. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 18, 747– 54. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Paavonen, E. J. , Aronen, E. T. , Moilanen, I., , Piha, J. , Rasanen, E. , Tamminen, T. et al. (2000). Sleep Problems of School-Aged Children: A Complementary View. Acta Paediatrica, 89, 223– 228. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pollock, J. I. (1992). Predictors and longterm associations of reported sleeping difficulties in infancy. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 10, 151– 168. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Prehn-Kristensen, A. , Goder, R. , Chirobeja, S. , Bressmann, I. , Ferstl, R. , Baving, L. (2009). Sleep in children enhances preferentially emotional declarative but not procedural memories. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104, 132– 139. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Quach, J. , Hiscock, H. , Ukoumunne, O. C. , Wake, M. (2011). A brief sleep intervention improves outcomes in the school entry year: a randomized controlled trial. Pediatrics, 128, 692– 701. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Quach, J. , Hiscock, H. , Canterford, L. , Wake, M. (2009). Outcomes of child sleep problems over the school-transition period: Australian population longitudinal study. Pediatrics, 123, 1287– 1292. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rasch, B. , Buchel, C. , Gais, S. , Born, J. (2007). Odor cues during slow-wave sleep prompt declarative memory consolidation. Science, 315, 1426– 1429. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sadeh, A. (2007). Consequences of Sleep Loss or Sleep Disruption in Children.. Sleep Medicine Clinics, 2, 513– 520. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sadeh, A. , Gruber, R. , Raviv, A. (2003). The effects of sleep restriction and extension on school-age children: what a difference an hour makes. Child Development, 74, 444– 445. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sadeh, A. , Gruber, R. , Raviv, A. (2002). Sleep, neurobehavioral functioning, and behavior problems in school-age children. Child Development, 73, 405– 17. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schabus, M. , Hoedlmoser, K. , Gruber, G. , Sauter, C. , Anderer, P. , Klösch, G. et al. (2006). Sleep spindle-related activity in the human EEG and its relation to general cognitive and learning abilities. European Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 1738– 1746. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schacter, D. L. (1987). Implicit memory: History and current status. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 501– 518. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schlarb, A. , Velten-Schurian, K. , Poets, C. R. , Hautzinger, M. (2011). First effects of a multicomponent treatment for sleep disorders in children. Nature and Science of Sleep, 3, 1– 11. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Schlarb, A. A. , Schwerdtle, B. , Hautzinger, M. (2010). Validation and psychometric properties of the German Version of the Children's Sleep Habits Questionnaire (CSHQ-DE). Somnologie, 14, 260– 266. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schlarb, A. A. , Hautzinger, M. (2009). Schlafstörungen in der Kindheit – psychische Störung im Erwachsenenalter?. Neuro-aktuell, 22, 23– 25. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Schneider, F. , Fink, G. (2007). Funktionelle MRT in Psychiatrie und Neurologie. Heidelberg.: Springer-Verlag. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schwerdtle, B. , Roeser, K. , Kübler, A. , Schlarb, A. A. (2010). Validation and psychometric properties of the German version of the Sleep Self Report (SSR-DE). A self-report questionnaire for the assessment of sleep disorders in children from 7 – 12 years old. Somnologie, 14, 267– 274. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Smedje, H. , Broman, J. E. , Hetta, J. (2001). Associations between disturbed sleep and behavioural difficulties in 635 children aged six to eight years: a study based on parents' perceptions. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 10, 1– 9. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Soffer-Dudek, N. , Sadeh, A. , Dahl, R. E. , Rosenblat-Stein, S. (2011). Poor sleep quality predicts deficient emotion information processing over time in early adolescence. Sleep, 34, 1499– 1508. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Steenari, M. R. , Vuontela, V. , Paavonen, E. J. , Carlson, S. , Fjallberg, M. , Aronen, E. (2003). Working memory and sleep in 6- to 13-year-old schoolchildren. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 42, 85– 92. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Taras, H. , Potts-Datema, W. (2005). Sleep and student performance at school. Journal of School Health, 75, 248– 254. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tononi, G. , Cirelli, C. (2003). Sleep and synaptic homeostasis: a hypothesis. Brain Research Bulletin, 62, 143– 150. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Velten-Schurian, K. , Hautzinger, M. , Poets, C. , Schlarb, A. (2010). Association between sleep patterns and daytime functioning in children with insomnia: The contribution of parent-reported frequency of night waking and wake time after sleep onset. Sleep Medicine, 11, 281– 288. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vriend, J. L. , Davidson, F. D. , Corkum, P. V. , Rusak, B. , McLaughlin, E. N. , Chambers, C. T. (2012). Sleep quantity and quality in relation to daytime functioning in children. Children's Health Care, 41, 204– 222. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Walker, M. P. , Stickgold, R. (2006). Sleep, Memory, and Plasticity. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 139– 166. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Walker, M. P. , Stickgold, R. , Alsop, D. , Gaab, N. , Schlaug, G. (2005a). Sleep-dependent motor memory plasticity in the human brain. Neuroscience, 133, 911– 17. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Walker, M. P. , Stickgold, R. , Jolesz, F. A. , Yoo, S.-S. (2005b). The functional anatomy of sleep-dependent visual skill learning. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1666– 75. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Walker, M. P. (2005c). A refined model of sleep and the time course of memory formation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 51– 104. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wilhelm, I. , Prehn-Kristensen, A. , Born, J. (2012). Sleep-dependent memory consolidation--what can be learnt from children?. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36, 1718– 1728. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wilhelm, I. , Diekelmann, S. , Born, J. (2008). Sleep in children improves memory performance on declarative but not procedural tasks. Learning & Memory, 15, 373– 377. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wolfson, A. R. , Carskadon, M. A. (2003). Understanding adolescents' sleep patterns and school performance: a critical appraisal. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 7, 491– 506. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar