Skip to main content
Open AccessMultistudy Report

Psychometric Properties of the Multifaceted Gender-Related Attributes Survey (GERAS)

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000528

Abstract

Abstract. Since the 1920s, psychologists have sought to assess the sex- and gender-related attributes of men and women, including primarily aspects of personality and focusing on positive characteristics. In this paper, we introduce a new questionnaire for assessing gender-related attributes with a broader approach than provided by previous ones. Therefore, the questionnaire includes (a) not only personality traits but also cognitions and interests and (b) not only positive but also negative and neutral characteristics. Two independent datasets were acquired (Study 1: N = 1,466; Study 2: N = 471) for development and psychometric analyses. Factor analysis confirmed a hierarchical structure with two separate dimensions of masculinity and femininity overarching the multiple first-order domains of personality, cognition, and interests. Analyses of reliability and convergence with other gender identity and personality scales revealed sufficient values. The new instrument discriminated between the biological sexes and was related to the gender quotas in participants’ occupations and social environments, thus providing evidence for criterion-related validity. Therefore, we propose the Gender-Related Attributes Survey (GERAS) as a useful tool for objectively assessing gender-related attributes across multiple facets in gender and sex-difference research.

Since the 1920s, psychologists have aimed to assess sex- and gender-related psychological attributes of men and women (for a summary, see Beere, 1990). The emerging differentiation between sex, reflecting the biological basis, and gender, reflecting shared cultural aspects, further led to increasing interest in these constructs (e.g., Dean & Tate, 2017). A number of related constructs have been differentiated such as gender roles or gender role identity. The former “refer to social and behavioral norms that, within a specific culture, are widely considered to be socially appropriate for individuals of a specific sex” (UN Women Training Centre eLearning Campus, 2018, para. 22). The latter emerges when individuals incorporate these cultural meanings into their self-concept, mostly used synonymously with gender role orientation, gender role self-concept, or gender-related self (see Guimond et al., 2007; Stewart & McDermott, 2004). Gender role identity terms a person’s identification with cultural definitions of being female and male or typically masculine or feminine attributes – independent of a person’s actual biological sex. The construct has been identified as an important characteristic in different fields: For example, higher masculinity in both, adolescent boys and girls was related to fewer depressive symptoms in health and developmental research (Priess, Lindberg, & Hyde, 2009), or higher scores on both masculinity and femininity were related to positive self-concept in Black male adolescents in racial/cultural research (Buckley, 2018).

In the late 1980s, Beere (1990) selected over 200 measures on gender roles, of which 28 were constructed to assess gender identity in both men and women. As we describe below, this number is even higher today. However, as Beere already criticized the authors did usually not sufficiently provide the psychometric properties of these measures, especially regarding coefficients of reliability and validity. Wood and Eagly (2015) recently distinguished two approaches for assessing gender role identity in psychological research: The first, older tradition was developed on the basis of research on individual differences in personality and interests. Gender-stereotypic personality traits and gender-typed interests (e.g., Lippa, 1991) represented this approach. Early attempts (e.g., Terman & Miles, 1936) suggested a unidimensional, bipolar construct of masculinity and femininity (e.g., Smiler, 2004) in this branch of research. However, newer studies – in which the personality traits communion and agency have commonly represented femininity and masculinity (Abele et al., 2016; Bem, 1974) – have suggested that masculinity and femininity form two separate dimensions. As a consequence, a person does not have to be categorized as either (a) masculine or (b) feminine, but – if classification is the goal – can also be classified as (c) androgynous, characterized by high scores on both the Masculinity and Femininity scales, or (d) undifferentiated, characterized by low scores on both scales (see Bem, 1977). A second, newer tradition of research and the assessment of gender role identity relates to the social identity perspective. Items have been designed to assess individuals’ feeling of belonging to the social category of women or men (Wood & Eagly, 2015). This newer approach has led to further developments that have also included indirect reaction time measures (e.g., van Well, Kolk, & Oei, 2007).

With respect to the first tradition, the most influential and widely applied early questionnaires were the Personal Attributes Questionnaire (PAQ; Spence, Helmreich, & Stapp, 1974) and the Bem Sex Role Inventory (BSRI; Bem, 1974). Both were designed primarily to integrate socially desirable items targeting agency and communion and were criticized for their unclear factor structure (Choi & Fuqua, 2003), low stability in recent decades (Twenge, 1997), and their poor applicability to different cultures (Colley, Mulhern, Maltby, & Wood, 2009; Hill, Fekken, & Bond, 2000). Newer approaches based on the BSRI and PAQ added negatively valued personality traits (PN-SRI; Berger & Krahé, 2013; PAQ-M/PAQ-F; Spence, Helmreich, & Holahan, 1979). A newer brief scale, the Traditional Masculinity-Femininity scale (TMF; Kachel, Steffens, & Niedlich, 2016), assesses the agreement between what an individual thinks the social norm is and his/her own position with respect to that on only six items. The brevity of this measure, albeit including four aspects of gender role identity beyond personality characteristics, represents advantages. However, scales consisting of so few items, on the one hand, require respondents to be capable of summing up all the necessary information to answer a rather broad item, and on the other hand, run the risk of having low reliability (Raykov, 2008).

To summarize, at present, the existing questionnaires for assessing gender role identity focus on more or less socially desirable aspects of personality (BSRI, PAQ), gender-related interests (gender diagnosticity; Lippa, 1991), or directly address the criteria with single items (TMF; sex role self-concept [SIS]; Pletzer, Petasis, Ortner, & Cahill, 2015). However, a literature search did not identify any instrument that combines these characteristics because they usually focus on one domain. To expand the choices of gender role identity measures, our new questionnaire was designed (a) to comprise a set of contemporary items that empirically reflect the current indications of research, (b) to apply sound methodology for scale construction, and (c) to implement aspects of personality, cognition, and interests into one measure.

With the present paper, we therefore present the development and psychometric properties of a new self-report questionnaire for assessing gender-related attributes: the Gender-Related Attributes Survey (GERAS). We conducted two studies to investigate the measure’s characteristics and to develop the present version of the questionnaire: In Study 1, we employed an online sample for the scale construction, factor analyses and first investigations of reliability and construct validity. In Study 2, we applied the thus resulting shortened version of the questionnaire as paper-pencil instrument in the lab, in order to cross validate the factor structure, and to investigate the new measure’s reliability, stability, and validity further. We essentially repeated the procedure from Study 1 but added more criteria for the analyses of criterion validity.

Study 1 – Scale Construction and Prevalidation

Method

Procedure

Participants were invited to take part in an online survey presented in the survey panel Unipark (by Questback GmbH, see https://my.unipark.com/) through Internet panels and mailing lists at German, Austrian, and Swiss universities and colleges. We promoted the questionnaire as an investigation of their self-report on several dimensions and whose relation to several biological and social variables. First, participants provided demographic information. Second, the first version of the GERAS was presented with all items in a randomized order, and the predominant gender in participants’ social environments was addressed. Third, participants were instructed to either rate the personality items regarding their relevance (How relevant is this item for describing gender differences?), stereotyping (How stereotypical is this item for a men/women?), social desirability (How socially desirable is this characteristic?). These ratings were conducted on Likert-scales from 1 (= not at all […]) to 7 (= very […]) for a preselection of the items further used for factor analyses, for results see Electronic Supplementary Material 1 (ESM 1). Alternatively, they filled out construct-related questionnaires such as the BSRI and PAQ, the Freiburg Personality Inventory (FPI-R; Fahrenberg, Hampel, & Selg, 2001), and the 16 Personality Factors Questionnaire (16PF-R; Schneewind & Graf, 1998). The subsample employed for estimating the measure’s test–retest correlations filled out the GERAS a second time after a period of 2–8 weeks. Thus, the number of items presented differed between the resulting subsamples and duration time ranged from 15 to 45 min, depending on the survey version. Table 1 presents the sample size, gender distribution, and age of the subsamples per each research goal.

Table 1 Sizes of subsamples, gender distributions, and Mage in Studies 1 and 2

Sample

A sample of 1,466 participants recruited from Austria (48.5%) and Germany (50.2%; 1.3% were native German speakers with other nationalities) completed the survey consisting of 399 men between 15 and 81 years of age (M = 27.47, SD = 10.19) and 1,067 women between 15 and 77 years (M = 23.87, SD = 6.65). As their highest educational level, 2% of the sample had completed primary school, 6% secondary school, 8% professional school, 59% A-levels, and 25% had a University degree. Participants were mostly students (about 70%) who had completed their A-levels (84%). About 30% of the participants were recruited from the general population from posts in online panels (e.g., under the topics sports or cooking).

Material

GERAS

The original version of the GERAS consisted of 100 items representing aspects of personality (formulated as single-word expressions), 24 items representing cognitive abilities, and 40 items representing activities and interests (both formulated as short-term statements). This item pool was based on (a) previous research literature examining (perceived) sex/gender differences in personality, cognitive abilities, or interests, and (b) a previous study exploring and clustering characteristics that participants named as relevant for the classification of their gender role identity (Pletzer et al., 2015).

The resulting first subscale focused on aspects of personality (Personality subscale, PS) that are culturally believed to be more present in women than in men or vice versa. Individuals were asked to rate themselves in relation to a general population of other men and women, in reference to the culture they predominantly associated themselves with. Besides the general positive communal and agentic personality traits (Abele et al., 2016; Bem, 1974), feminine items included aspects such as frequency of emotional expressions (e.g., Feingold, 1994) and intuition (e.g, Fischer, 1993) as well as contents obtained from the Big Five personality traits neuroticism and agreeableness (Schmitt, Realo, Voracek, & Allik, 2008). Masculine items addressed ascribed rationality (Fischer, 1993), risk-taking behavior (Byrnes, Miller, & Schafer, 1999), and competitiveness (Lynn, 1993). The second subscale (Cognition subscale, CS) focused on cognitive abilities that are typically seen as more pronounced in either men or women (for a review, see Andreano & Cahill, 2009; Eccles, Freedman-Doan, Frome, Jacobs, & Yoon, 2000). Participants were asked to rate the anticipated difficulty of performing certain tasks (e.g., “finding the right words” [feminine] or “finding one’s way” [masculine]). The masculine dimension of the GERAS further comprised abilities such as visuospatial processing, navigation, or logical/mathematical skills. The feminine dimension of the first version of the GERAS included verbal ability, autobiographical memory, or the ability to memorize faces and names. For the third subscale, the Activities and Interests Subscale (AIS), participants were asked to rate their desire to pursue certain activities. In line with previous studies (e.g., Dietz-Uhler, Harrick, End, & Jacquemotte, 2000; Evans, Schweingruber, & Stevenson, 2002), activities considered feminine lay within the areas of language and arts, make-up/clothing, and social activities (e.g., “talking on the phone”), as well as sports that are considered to be rather calm in nature (e.g., “dancing”). Masculine items mostly focused on competitive, team-related, or comparatively action-packed sports and activities (e.g., “pursuing ball-sports”, “playing cards”).

Related Measures of Gender Role Identity

We employed the BSRI-F (Femininity; 20 items, e.g., “romantic”; ω = .88) and BSRI-M (Masculinity; 20 items, e.g., “fearless”; ω = .92) scales on a 7-point Likert-scale ranging from 1 (= does not apply to me) to 7 (= mostly applies to me). Furthermore, we employed the PAQ-M (Masculinity/Instrumentality; eight items, e.g., “active”; ω = .81) and the PAQ-F (Femininity/Expressivity; eight items, e.g., “friendly”; ω = .86) scales on a 5-point Likert-scale ranging from 1 (= does not apply to me) to 5 (= mostly applies to me).

Personality Questionnaire Subscales

We employed the subscales Dominance, Emotional Stability, Warmth, Sensitivity, and Concern from the 16PF-R (.77 ≤ ω ≥ .84), as well as the FPI-R (.69 ≤ ω ≥ .82) to assess Achievement Orientation, Aggressiveness, Irritability, and Social Orientation.

Other Criteria
Social Environment

We assessed participants’ usual social environment with eight items from the newly developed Peer Relations Scale (PRS; male-PRS: ω = .75 and female-PRS: ω = .73). Items addressed the sex of important self-reported attachment figures (“Are there mainly women/men among your closest attachment figures?”), sex of social environment while growing up (Did you grow up with many women/men?), sex of current friends, and self-reported general feelings of how one gets along with women and men. Ratings were given on a 7-point Likert-scale ranging from 1 (= do not agree at all) to 7 (= agree very much).

Occupational Gender (OCG)

Participants indicated their current profession or professional education. Professions were clustered into a scale ranging from 1 (= very masculine profession) to 7 (= very feminine profession) in accordance with the gender distribution of the profession or branch (Institut für Arbeitsmarkt- und Berufsforschung – IAB, 2010).

Demographic Data

We further assessed participants’ age, nationality, first language, and educational background.

Statistical Analyses

Missing values in both datasets were imputed by applying the Multivariate Imputation by Chained Equation (MICE) Algorithm implemented in SPSS 24. Further analyses were carried out with R (version 3.5.0) using the packages “lavaan” (version 0.6-1; Rosseel, 2018), “nFactors” (version 2.3.3; Raiche & Magis, 2010) and “psych” (version 1.6.4; Revelle, 2016).

To investigate the factor structure in a first step, we split the Study 1 dataset employing Subset 1 for exploratory purposes and Subset 2 to cross-validate these findings (and further the dataset from Study 2). In the exploratory part, we first used EFA to set the number of factors to find a model separately for the three subscales PS, CS and AIS. We excluded items with (a) loadings < .30 on any factor or (b) loadings > .30 on more than one factor. We then cross-validated this factor solution. Second, we tested measurement invariance across men and women for these factor solutions of the subscales in all datasets. Finally, we investigated the hierarchical factor structure modeling latent variables based on the previous factor analyses (see ESM 2 for a more detailed description of our modeling approach).

We investigated internal consistency of the GERAS subscales by calculating omega, and further calculated split-half and retest reliability. Masculinity and femininity scores from related measures as well as the 16PF-R and FPI-R scales served as criteria for analyses on convergent validity. In order to evaluate criterion-related validity, we analyzed sex differences for all GERAS scales, and the relation to the PRS items and occupational classification. Data and R scripts are published in the Open Science Framework (see https://osf.io/42jhr/).

Results

Factor Structure

First-Order Models

For PS, EFA and CFAs revealed a final five-factor model including 10 masculine and 10 feminine items. The feminine items loaded on two factors that we named “expressivity” and “neuroticism”; the masculine items were grouped in three factors “risk-taking,” “assertiveness,” and “rationality” (see Table S1 for item loadings and Table S4 for the model fits from the EFA and CFAs in ESM 3). For CS, the factor analysis revealed a final four-factor model including seven masculine and seven feminine items. Masculine items accumulated into “spatial abilities” and “numerical abilities”; feminine items accumulated into “verbal abilities” and “memory functions” (see Table S2 for item loadings in ESM 3). For AIS, factor analyses revealed a model including eight masculine and eight feminine items, which congregated into masculine and feminine “social interests” and “sports interests” factors (see Table S3 for item loadings in ESM 3). For these first-order models, all datasets revealed measurement invariance across men and women (Tables S1–S3, ESM 4).

Higher-Order Model

Investigations of the higher-order facture structure revealed best model fit indices for a third-order model (χ2[66, N = 733] = 88.83, CFI = .944, RMSEA = .022, SRMR = .092, χ2/df = 1.35; for the fit indices of all three tested higher-order models, see Table S5 in ESM 3). Within this final model, the first-order factors loaded on subscale-conform second-order Personality, Cognition, and Activities and Interests factors per each third-order factor Masculinity and Femininity. In order to obtain model fit, we allowed for error covariance between the first-order factors risk-taking and neuroticism, and between the second-order factors of masculine and feminine cognitions. This factor solution was confirmed in Subset 2 in Study 1 (χ2[66, N = 733] = 98.26, CFI = .924, RMSEA = .026, SRMR = .092, χ2/df = 1.49, displayed in Figure 1).

Figure 1 Factor loadings and allowed covariances in Model 3 for testing the third-order model of the GERAS. Indices are presented for three subsets Study 1 Subset 1/Study 1 Subset 2/Study 2. The third-order factors Masculinity (M) and Femininity (F) each overarched the three second-order factors Personality (P-M/P-F), Activities and Interests (AI-M/-F), and Cognitions (C-M/-F). P-M consisted of first-order factors Risk-Taking (PRT), Assertiveness (PA), and Rationality (PR), P-F consisted of Expressivity (PE) and Neuroticism (PN). AI-M and AI-F consisted in Interests Sports (IspM/IspF) and Interests Social (IsoM/IsoF), respectively. C-M comprised spatial (CSp) and numerical (CN) cognition factors, C-F comprised memory (CM) and verbal (CV) cognition factors.

Reliability Analyses

Revelle’s Omega, Split-half coefficients (rttα), and retest correlation coefficients (rtt) were sufficient for both the GERAS-Masculinity (ω = .85, rttα = .87, rtt = .88) and GERAS-Femininity (ω = .86, rttα = .85, rtt = .80) scales.

Convergent Validity

For detailed results regarding subscales (Table S1) and calculated separated by sex (Table S2), see ESM 6.

Convergence With Other Gender Role Identity Scales

GERAS-Masculinity was significantly related to the BSRI-M (r = .44, p < .01) and PAQ-M (r = .35, p < .01) scales, whereas GERAS-Femininity was significantly related to the BSRI-F (r = .50, p < .01) and PAQ-F (r = .58, p < .01) scales. A similar pattern emerged when calculated for men and women separately.

Convergence With Other Personality Scales

GERAS-Masculinity was significantly positively related to the FPI-R score for Achievement Orientation (r = .41, p < .01) and the 16PF-R scores for Dominance (r = .27, p < .01) and Emotional Stability (r = .26, p < .01). GERAS-Femininity was positively related to the FPI-R score for Social Orientation (r = .22, p > .05 after Holm correction) and the 16PF-R scores for Warmth (r = .33, p < .01), Sensitivity (r = .26, p < .01), and Concern (r = .16, p > .05). A somewhat similar pattern emerged when calculated separated by sex.

Criterion-Related Validity

Biological Sex

Men scored significantly higher on GERAS-Masculinity (M = 4.49, SD = 0.72) than women (M = 3.68, SD = 0.67, F[1, 1464] = 404.23, p > .001), and women scored significantly higher on GERAS-Femininity (M = 4.80, SD = 0.60) than men (M = 4.04, SD = 0.62, F[1, 1464] = 462.23, p < .001).

Social Environment

GERAS-Masculinity and GERAS-Femininity were both significantly positively related to self-reports of growing up with men/women (all rs ≥ .23, all ps < .01), having male/female attachment figures during childhood (all rs ≥ .22, all ps < .01), having mostly male/female friends (all rs ≥ .29, all ps < .001), and getting along with men/women (all rs ≥ .17, all ps < .01). When analyzed separately for men and women, a similar pattern emerged (all rs ≥ .14, all ps < .01), except for the relation between GERAS-Femininity and growing up with women, which disappeared in men (r = .11, p > .05).

OCG

Correlation analysis between OCG and GERAS-Masculinity revealed a negative coefficient (r = −.32, p < .01), indicating that participants scoring higher on GERAS-Masculinity were employed in professions predominantly chosen by men. GERAS-Femininity was significantly positively related to OCG (r = .28, p < .01), indicating that participants scoring higher on GERAS-Femininity were employed in professions predominantly chosen by women. Separate analyses for men and women did not reveal significant correlation coefficients.

Study 2 – Validation

Method

Procedure and Material

Participants were recruited from courses and through announcement boards at the University of Salzburg and through bulletin boards in civic centers. Participants first worked on a demographic questionnaire, followed by a shortened paper-pencil version of the GERAS (20 PS items, 14 CS items and 16 AIS items; see ESM 5 for the whole questionnaire) based on results from Study 1. Except for scales of 16PF-R and FPI-R, additional instruments were presented as in Study 1 (BSRI-F: ω = .77; BSRI-M: ω = .90; PAQ-M: ω = .79; PAQ-F: ω = .85; male-PRS: ω = .76; female-PRS: ω = .74). Additionally, participants were asked to indicate how masculine or feminine (ranging from 1 = not at all masculine/feminine to 9 = very masculine/feminine) they considered themselves compared with men, women, and the total population on a six-item-scale (SIS; Pletzer et al., 2015; three items each; SIS-Masculinity: ω = .94; SIS-Femininity: ω = .94). They were further asked to also have a close friend or relative rate them on the SIS (SIS-Masculinity-other: ω = .96; SIS-Femininity-other: ω = .96).

Sample

Participants were 471 native German speakers (230 men, Mage = 26.11, SD = 8.86; 241 women, Mage = 25.40, SD = 8.98) who completed the questionnaire (primary school: 0.4%; secondary school: 3%; professional school: 4.5%; A-levels: 83%; university degree: 9%). Most of them were students (about 70%) who had completed their A-levels (92%). About 30% of the participants were recruited from the general population.

Statistical Analyses

Statistical analyses were similar to Study 1, adding the SIS scales in convergent validity analyses. Furthermore, we investigated incremental validity in multiple regression analysis including the BSRI, PAQ, and GERAS scales as predictors of OCG.

Results

Factor Structure

Data from Study 2 confirmed the results of Study 1 regarding the factor structure of the GERAS by showing good model fit (χ2[66, N = 471] = 35.59, CFI = 1.00, RMSEA < .001, SRMR = .088, χ2/df = 0.54) in the cross-validation.

Reliability Analyses

Revelle’s Omega, split-half coefficients (rttα), and retest correlation coefficients (rtt) were sufficient for both the GERAS-Masculinity (ω = .86; rttα = .88; rtt = .90) and GERAS-Femininity (ω = .88; rttα = .88; rtt = .93) scales. Tables 2 and 3 present additional results of item analyses (reliability coefficients per scale [and facet], item selectivity, and item difficulties).

Table 2 Psychometric properties (Revelle’s Omega, corrected item-total correlations, and item difficulties) for the GERAS-masculinity scales and items from Study 2
Table 3 Psychometric properties (Revelle’s Omega, corrected item-total correlations, and item difficulties) for the GERAS-Femininity scales and items from Study 2

Convergent Validity

Relation With Other Gender Role Identity Scales

As Table S3 in ESM 6 indicates in detail per GERAS subscales, correlation coefficients ranging from .43 to .57 for GERAS-Masculinity and from .59 to .65 for GERAS-Femininity confirmed convergent validity regarding relations with the BSRI, PAQ, and SIS scales. Separate analyses for men and women revealed a slightly different pattern, however: GERAS-Femininity was not significantly related to SIS-Femininity in men (Table S4). Further, GERAS-Masculinity was not significantly related to BSRI-M and PAQ-M scores, and GERAS-Femininity to PAQ-F score in women (Table S5).

Multiple regression analyses for incremental validity revealed that GERAS-Masculinity was a significant predictor of participants’ OCG, explaining 12% more variance (R2 = .18, p < .001) than the BSRI-M and PAQ-M did together (R2 = .06, p < .05). Similarly, GERAS-Femininity explained 13% more variance when predicting OCG (R2 = .22, p < .001) than the BSRI-F and PAQ-F did (R2 = .09, p < .01; for details, see Table S6 in ESM 6).

Criterion-Related Validity

Biological Sex

Men scored significantly higher on GERAS-Masculinity (M = 4.56, SD = 0.68) than women (M = 3.89, SD = 0.65, F[1, 469] = 118.18, p < .001), and women scored significantly higher on GERAS-Femininity (M = 4.94, SD = 0.54) than men (M = 4.15, SD = 0.56, F[1, 469] = 238.38, p < .001; see Figure 2).

Figure 2 Sex differences for GERAS-Scales from Study 2 showing men scoring significantly higher on GERAS-Masculinity scales and women scoring sgnificantly higher on GERAS-Femininity scales (all ps < .001). Error bars indicate standard errors. M/F indicates masculinity/femiminity scales.
Gender Role Evaluation by Others

The perception of participants’ SIS-Masculinity and SIS-Femininity by others was significantly related to the GERAS-Masculinity and GERAS-Femininity across all participants (Masculinity: r = .49, p < .01; Femininity: r = .54, p < .01). However, this relation disappeared when tested separately for men and women (all rs ≤ .18; all ps > .05).

Social Environment

GERAS-Masculinity and GERAS-Femininity were both significantly positively related to self-reports of growing up with men/women (both rs ≥ .19, both ps < .01), having male/female attachment figures during childhood (both rs ≥ .27, both ps < .01), having mostly male/female friends (both rs ≥ .29, both ps < .01), and getting along with men/women (both rs = .14, both ps < .01). When analyzed separately for men and women, a somewhat similar pattern emerged (all rs ≥ .19, all ps < .01), except for the association between GERAS-Femininity and female attachment figures, and growing up with women (rs ≤ .12, both ps > .05), and between GERAS-Masculinity and getting along with men (r = .13, p > .05) in the women sample.

OCG

Correlation analysis between OCG and GERAS-Masculinity revealed a negative coefficient (r = −.41, p < .01), indicating that participants with higher GERAS-Masculinity scores were employed in professions predominantly chosen by men. GERAS-Femininity was significantly positively related to OCG (r = .46, p < .01), indicating that participants with higher GERAS-Femininity scores were employed in professions predominantly chosen by women. Separate analyses for men and women did not reveal significant correlation coefficients, except for the relation to the GERAS-Femininity scale in men, indicating that men with higher GERAS-Femininity scores tended to be employed in professions predominantly chosen by women (r = .37, p < .01).

Discussion

These studies were conducted to construct a contemporary and multidimensional measure for the self-assessment of gender role identity. In three steps, a large item pool was reduced to a 50-item instrument in which participants were instructed to rate the extent to which they attributed gender-related traits, activities, and interests to themselves. The instrument stands out against previous approaches by (a) including not only personality traits but also cognitions and interests, (b) including not only agentic and communal but also other gender-typical personality traits, and (c) including not only positive but also negative and neutral traits. The data revealed that all items included in the final instrument differentiated between men and women, and stereotypical views of men and women.

Three independent datasets revealed a similar factor structure. Results confirmed the proposed multifaceted structure of the GERAS with several masculine and feminine first-order factors in PS, CS, and AIS. Interestingly, not only expressivity and assertiveness arose as meaningful first-order factors in PS, but also neuroticism, risk-taking, and rationality, suggesting that gender-related attributes indeed include more than two aspects of personality. Most important, however, analyses revealed an overall two-dimensional model with Masculinity and Femininity as separate third-order factors. We allowed correlations between the first-order factors risk-taking and neuroticism, and between second-order factors feminine and masculine cognition. The former is legitimate as risk-taking is defined as a facet of extraversion, which is expected to be opposite to neuroticism (e.g., Nicholson, Soane, Fenton‐O’Creevy, & Willman, 2005). The latter also is explainable, as the feminine cognition factors verbal and memory skills seem to be rather basic cognitive capacities that may explain general cognitive skills to a certain extent and are not specifically feminine compared with spatial and numerical skills on the masculine side. Moreover, as the correlations were not too high across all three datasets, we left them included.

However, because we had constructed the instrument following the two-dimensional approach of gender role identity, one could argue that it is not surprising that the analyses of the data confirmed this structure. Nevertheless, we aimed to assess gender role identity on multiple facets building a base for masculinity and femininity, but we did not focus on only these third-order constructs. Furthermore, in our exploratory factor analyses, we tested a one-dimensional model with one gender role factor overarching all first-order factors in the factor analyses. However, the final two-dimensional outperformed this one-dimensional model, which did not meet the cutoff values set for model fit indices. Our results not only support the assessment of masculinity and femininity as separate dimensions (see Bem, 1974) but also across multiple domains (cf. Choi & Fuqua, 2003; Diekman & Eagly, 2000). As follows, we recommend to calculate the GERAS scores by first averaging the items per masculine and feminine PS, CS, and AIS, and second, by averaging these subscale scores for each a GERAS-Masculinity and -Femininity score.

In contrast to other gender role identity scales, the factor structure revealed measurement invariance across men and women because we excluded the highly stereotypical items referring to body strength or beauty based on the modification indices.

The analyses furthermore suggested good to high internal consistency, split-half, and retest correlation coefficients, indicating the measure’s reliability. Medium to high relations between the GERAS scores and other gender role identity scales (BSRI, PAQ, SIS), and significant relations with personality scale scores for assessing constructs such as dominance, emotional stability, social orientation, and sensitivity provided evidence for convergent and criterion-related validity. GERAS-Masculinity was related to SIS-Masculinity in both men and women and GERAS-Femininity to SIS-Femininity in women, and only slightly in men. A possible explanation for the latter may be the deviation of SIS-Femininity from the less direct femininity assessment via the GERAS that may have resulted from a greater acceptance of and desire for masculine traits in women as opposed to feminine traits in men (cf. Johnson, Murphy, Zewdie, & Reichard, 2008). The fact that male participants showed mean GERAS-Femininity scores above the scale midpoint on PS and CS supported this idea. In other words, when asked directly how feminine they were, men might not have indicated scores that were as high as when asked in a less direct way via the attributes concerning personality or cognition from the GERAS. Given that men’s values were rather low on feminine activities and interests scale, this facet seems to be viewed as fairly undesirable by men.

Furthermore, both GERAS scales provided an increased invariance explained in gender quota of participants’ occupation above and beyond the amount explained by the BSRI and PAQ scores. These results support our assumption that gender identity bases on more facets than only the ones accounted for by personality characteristics as our measure outperformed previous well-established instruments. Besides the fact that the GERAS-Masculinity and GERAS-Femininity scales discriminated between men and women as predetermined by the item construction, results further revealed evidence for criterion-related validity. Specifically, the GERAS scores were related to the gender quotas for participants’ occupations on the one hand and to their social environment on the other hand. However, biological sex may have affected these relations because some of the relations disappeared when calculated separately for men and women. Interestingly, for men, GERAS-Femininity remained significantly related to occupations with a higher quota of women. This may indicate that men tend to choose or prefer professions that match their individual characteristics. Conversely, sex stereotypes may be more relevant for women because women tend to choose professions with higher quotas of women and tend to avoid professions with higher quotas of men (see Eagly & Karau, 2002; Garcia-Retamero & López-Zafra, 2006).

Evaluations of masculinity and femininity by others were related to the GERAS scores only in the total sample but not when analyzed separately for men and women. This suggests that, different from self-ratings, the biological sex of the targets may have strongly biased the others’ ratings.

However, our research was not free from limitations. First, samples of both studies were not representative, nor the total samples nor the hypothesis-specific subsamples. Therefore, our findings might not be generalizable. Second and relatedly, our results do not provide indications for a broader cultural context because our samples consisted of only German-speaking participants in Central Europe. Thus, future research should investigate the validity of the GERAS across cultures because gender role identity is a concept formed by cultural and social norms. For example, gender differences have been shown to be larger in Western nations (Guimond et al., 2007); therefore, the GERAS scales might not differentiate between the biological sexes in other cultures. Third, indications of our results might not hold for the long-term because the socially dependent dimensions of gender role identity might differ over time (Twenge, 1997) and might be influenced by social changes. Fourth, although the questionnaire instructions did not reveal the construct of interest and therefore may reduce biases with regard to a person’s gender role identity, social desirability might still be a limitation as is true for all self-report instruments (Morgado, Meireles, Neves, Amaral, & Ferreira, 2017). Fifth, as Morgado et al. (2017) pointed out, web-based surveys present a further limitation regarding the scale development process and therefore affect the methodology of Study 1. However, we accepted this aspect because we were able to obtain a larger sample size and to confirm the validity of the online data by employing a paper-pencil approach in Study 2.

In sum, these results provide comprehensive evidence for the reliability and validity of the GERAS. Thus, we propose that the GERAS is a useful tool for assessing gender role identity across multiple aspects in gender and sex-difference research. Future studies should investigate the stability of our measure in relation to a participant’s hormonal state and should evaluate additional aspects of criterion validity, such as correlations between the CS and structural/functional brain imaging, actual achievement in verbal and visuospatial performance tests, or the psychometric properties and validity of the English version of the GERAS for international usefulness.

The authors want to acknowledge that the project was in part funded by the Austrian Science Fund (P28261).

References

  • Abele, A. E., Hauke, N., Peters, K., Louvet, E., Szymkow, A., & Duan, Y. (2016). Facets of the fundamental content dimensions: Agency with competence and assertiveness – communion with warmth and morality. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1810. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01810 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Andreano, J. M., & Cahill, L. (2009). Sex influences on the neurobiology of learning and memory. Learning & Memory, 16, 248–266. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.918309 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Beere, C. A. (1990). Gender roles: A handbook of tests and measures. Westport, CT: Greenwood Publishing Group. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bem, S. L. (1974). The measurement to psychological androgyny. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42, 155–162. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0036215 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bem, S. L. (1977). On the utility of alternative procedures for assessing psychological androgyny. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 45, 196–205. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.45.2.196 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Berger, A., & Krahé, B. (2013). Negative attributes are gendered too: Conceptualizing and measuring positive and negative facets of sex-role identity. European Journal of Social Psychology, 43, 516–531. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.1970 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Buckley, T. R. (2018). Black adolescent males: Intersections among their gender role identity and racial identity and associations with self-concept (global and school). Child Development, 89, e311–e322. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12950 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Byrnes, J. P., Miller, D. C., & Schafer, W. D. (1999). Gender differences in risk taking: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 367–383. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.125.3.367 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Choi, N., & Fuqua, D. R. (2003). The structure of the Bem sex role inventory: A summary report of 23 validation studies. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 63, 872–887. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164403258235 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Colley, A., Mulhern, G., Maltby, J., & Wood, A. M. (2009). The short form BSRI: Instrumentality, expressiveness and gender associations among a United Kingdom sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 46, 384–387. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2008.11.005 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dean, M. L., & Tate, C. C. (2017). Extending the legacy of Sandra Bem: Psychological androgyny as a touchstone conceptual advance for the study of gender in psychological science. Sex Roles, 76, 643–654. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-016-0713-z First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Diekman, A. B., & Eagly, A. H. (2000). Stereotypes as dynamic constructs: Women and men of the past, present, and future. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1171–1188. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167200262001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dietz-Uhler, B., Harrick, E. A., End, C., & Jacquemotte, L. (2000). Sex differences in sport fan behavior and reasons for being a sport fan. Journal of Sport Behavior, 23, 219–232. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Eagly, A. H., & Karau, S. J. (2002). Role congruity theory of prejudice toward female leaders. Psychological Review, 109, 573–598. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.109.3.573 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Eccles, J. S., Freedman-Doan, C., Frome, P., Jacobs, J., & Yoon, K. S. (2000). Gender-role socialization in the family: A longitudinal approach. In T. EckesH. M. TrautnerEds., The developmental social psychology of gender. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Evans, E. M., Schweingruber, H., & Stevenson, H. W. (2002). Gender differences in interest and knowledge acquisition: The United States, Taiwan, and Japan. Sex Roles, 15, 153–167. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1021047122532 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fahrenberg, J., Hampel, R., & Selg, H. (2001). Das Freiburger Persönlichkeitsinventar FPI. Revidierte Fassung FPI-R und teilweise geänderte Fassung FPI-A1. Handanweisung 7. Auflage [The Freiburg Personality Inventory. Revised edition FPI-R and partially revised edition FPI-A1. Manual 7th ed.]. Germany, Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Feingold, A. (1994). Gender differences in personality: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 429–456. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.116.3.429 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fischer, A. H. (1993). Sex differences in emotionality: Fact or stereotype? Feminism & Psychology, 3, 303–318. https://doi.org/10.1177/0959353593033002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Garcia-Retamero, R., & López-Zafra, E. (2006). Prejudice against women in male-congenial environments: Perceptions of gender role congruity in leadership. Sex Roles, 55, 51–61. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-006-9068-1 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Guimond, S., Branscombe, N. R., Brunot, S., Buunk, A. P., Chatard, A., Désert, M., … Yzerbyt, V. (2007). Culture, gender, and the self: Variations and impact of social comparison processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 1118–1134. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.92.6.1118 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hill, S. A., Fekken, G. C., & Bond, S. L. (2000). Factor structure integrity of the personal attributes questionnaire: An English–French comparison. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 32, 234–242. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0087120 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Institut für Arbeitsmarkt-und Berufsforschung – IAB. (2010). Berufe im Spiegel der Statistik [Professions through the mirror of statistics]. Retrieved from https://de.wikipedia.org/wiki/Liste_von_Frauenanteilen_in_der_Berufswelt#cite_note-39 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Johnson, S. K., Murphy, S. E., Zewdie, S., & Reichard, R. J. (2008). The strong, sensitive type: Effects of gender stereotypes and leadership prototypes on the evaluation of male and female leaders. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 106, 39–60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2007.12.002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kachel, S., Steffens, M. C., & Niedlich, C. (2016). Traditional masculinity and femininity: Validation of a new scale assessing gender roles. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 956. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00956 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lippa, R. (1991). Some psychometric characteristics of gender diagnosticity measures: Reliability, validity, consistency across domains, and relationship to the big five. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 1000–1011. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.61.6.1000 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lynn, R. (1993). Sex differences in competitiveness and the valuation of money in twenty countries. The Journal of Social Psychology, 133, 507–511. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.1993.9712175 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Morgado, F. F. R., Meireles, J. F. F., Neves, C. M., Amaral, A. C. S., & Ferreira, M. E. C. (2017). Scale development: Ten main limitations and recommendations to improve future research practices. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 30, 1–20. https://doi.org/10.1186/s41155-016-0057-1 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Nicholson, N., Soane, E., Fenton‐O’Creevy, M., & Willman, P. (2005). Personality and domain‐specific risk taking. Journal of Risk Research, 8, 157–176. https://doi.org/10.1080/1366987032000123856 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pletzer, B., Petasis, O., Ortner, T. M., & Cahill, L. (2015). Interactive effects of culture and sex hormones on the sex role self-concept. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 9, 240. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2015.00240 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Priess, H. A., Lindberg, S. M., & Hyde, J. S. (2009). Adolescent gender-role identity and mental health: Gender intensification revisited. Child Development, 80, 1531–1544. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01349.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Raiche, G., & Magis, D. (2010). nFactors: Parallel analysis and non graphical solutions to the Cattell scree test (R package version 2.3.3). Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=nFactors First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Raykov, T. (2008). Alpha if item deleted: A note on loss of criterion validity loss in scale revision if maximizing coefficient alpha. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 61, 275–285. https://doi.org/10.1348/000711007X188520 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Revelle, W. (2016). Psych: Procedures for personality and psychological research (R package version 1.6.4). Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psych First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Rosseel, Y. (2018). Lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling (R package version 0.6-1). Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lavaan First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Schmitt, D. P., Realo, A., Voracek, M., & Allik, J. (2008). Why can’t a man be more like a woman? Sex differences in big five personality traits across 55 cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 168–182. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.94.1.168 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schneewind, K. A., & Graf, J. (1998). Deutsche Ausgabe des 16 PF [German version of the 16 PF] (22nd ed.). Bern, Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Smiler, A. P. (2004). Thirty years after the discovery of gender: Psychological concepts and measures of masculinity. Sex Roles, 50, 15–26. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:SERS.0000011069.02279.4c First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Spence, J. T., Helmreich, R. L., & Holahan, C. K. (1979). Negative and positive components of psychological masculinity and femininity and their relationships to self-reports of neurotic and acting out behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1673–1682. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.37.10.1673 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Spence, J. T., Helmreich, R. L., & Stapp, J. (1974). The personal attributes questionnaire: A measure of sex role stereotypes and masculinity-femininity. Journal Supplement Abstract Service Catalogue of Selected Documents in Psychology, 4, 43–44. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Stewart, A. J., & McDermott, C. (2004). Gender in psychology. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 519–544. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141537 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Terman, L. M., & Miles, C. C. (1936). Sex and personality: Studies in masculinity and femininity. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Twenge, J. M. (1997). Changes in masculine and feminine traits over time: A meta-analysis. Sex Roles, 36, 305–325. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02766650 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • UN Women Training Centre eLearning Campus. (2018). Gender equality glossary. Retrieved from https://trainingcentre.unwomen.org/mod/glossary/view.php?id=36&mode=letter&hook=G&sortkey=&sortorder=&fullsearch=0&page=0 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • van Well, S., Kolk, A. M., & Oei, N. Y. L. (2007). Direct and indirect assessment of gender role identification. Sex Roles, 56, 617–628. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-007-9203-7 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wood, W., & Eagly, A. H. (2015). Two traditions of research on gender identity. Sex Roles, 73, 461–473. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-015-0480-2 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

Freya Gruber, Department of Psychology, University of Salzburg, Hellbrunnerstr. 34, 5020 Salzburg, Austria, E-mail