Skip to main content
Original Articles and Reviews

Mind Reading Using Neuroimaging

Is This the Future of Deception Detection?

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040/a000193

Traditional lie detection tools, such as the polygraph, voice stress analysis, or special interrogation techniques, rely on behavioral or psychophysiological manifestations of deception. With the advent of neuroimaging techniques, the question emerged whether it would be possible to directly identify deceit in the part of the body where it is generated: the brain. After a few promising studies, these techniques became soon commercially available and there have been attempts to use such results in the court in recent years. The current article reviews the development of neuroimaging techniques in the field of deception detection and critically discusses the potential but also the shortcomings of such methods. Unfortunately, the majority of research in this field was rather unsystematic and neglected the accumulated knowledge regarding methodological pitfalls that were extensively discussed in the scientific community in conjunction with the polygraph. Therefore, neuroimaging studies on deception largely differ with respect to the experimental paradigm (the interrogation technique), the methods for analyzing the data, and the procedures to obtain individual diagnoses. Moreover, most studies used artificial laboratory settings that differ considerably from real-life applications. As a consequence, neuroimaging techniques are not applicable for detecting deception in individual field cases at the moment. However, recent advantages such as multivariate pattern analysis might yield novel neuroimaging applications in the near future that are capable of improving established techniques for detecting deception or concealed knowledge.

References

  • Abe, N., Okuda, J., Suzuki, M., Sasaki, H., Matsuda, T., Mori, E., … Fuji, T. (2008). Neural correlates of true memory, false memory, and deception. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 2811–2819. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Abe, N., Suzuki, M., Mori, E., Itoh, M., Fujii, T. (2007). Deceiving others: Distinct neural responses of the prefrontal cortex and amygdala in simple fabrication and deception with social interactions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 287–295. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Abe, N., Suzuki, M., Tsukiura, T., Mori, E., Yamaguchi, K., Itoh, M., Fujii, T. (2006). Dissociable roles of prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices in deception. Cerebral Cortex, 16, 192–199. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Aron, A. R., Robbins, T. W., Poldrack, R. A. (2004). Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 170–177. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ben-Shakhar, G. (2002). A critical review of the Control Question Test (CQT). In M. Kleiner (Ed.), Handbook of polygraph testing (pp. 103–126). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ben-Shakhar, G., Dolev, K. (1996). Psychophysiological detection through the guilty knowledge technique: Effects of mental countermeasures. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81, 273–281. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ben-Shakhar, G., Elaad, E. (2003). The validity of psychophysiological detection of information with the Guilty Knowledge Test: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 131–151. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ben-Shakhar, G., Furedy, J. J. (1990). Theories and applications in the detection of deception: A psychophysiological and international perspective. New York, NY: Springer. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bhatt, S., Mbwana, J., Adeyemo, A., Sawyer, A., Hailu, A., Vanmeter, J. (2009). Lying about facial recognition: An fMRI study. Brain and Cognition, 69, 382–390. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.08.033 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bles, M., Haynes, J.-D. (2008). Detecting concealed information using brain-imaging technology. Neurocase, 14, 82–92. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Buchsbaum, B. R., Lemire-Rodger, S., Fang, C., Abdi, H. (2012). The neural basis of vivid memory is patterned on perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24, 1867–1883. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00253 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Christ, S. E., Van Essen, D. C., Watson, J. M., Brubaker, L. E., McDermott, K. B. (2009). The contributions of prefrontal cortex and executive control to deception: Evidence from activation likelihood estimate meta-analyses. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 1557–1566. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn189 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Corbetta, M., Patel, G., Shulman, G. L. (2008). The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to theory of mind. Neuron, 58, 306–324. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.04.017 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cui, X., Bray, S., Bryant, D. M., Glover, G. H., Reiss, A. L. (2011). A quantitative comparison of NIRS and fMRI across multiple cognitive tasks. NeuroImage, 54, 2808–2821. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.069 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Davatzikos, C., Ruparel, K., Fan, Y., Shen, D. G., Acharyya, M., Loughead, J. W., … Langleben, D. D. (2005). Classifying spatial patterns of brain activity with machine learning methods: Application to lie detection. NeuroImage, 28, 663–668. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Debey, E., Verschuere, B., Crombez, G. (2012). Lying and executive control: An experimental investigation using ego depletion and goal neglect. Acta Psychologica, 140, 133–141. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.03.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Deceiving the law . (2008). Nature Neuroscience, 11, 1231 doi: 10.1038/nn1108-1231 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ding, X. P., Du, X., Lei, D., Hu, C. S., Fu, G., Chen, G. (2012). The neural correlates of identity faking and concealment: An FMRI study. PLoS One, 7, e48639 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0048639 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Downar, J., Crawley, A. P., Mikulis, D. J., Davis, K. D. (2002). A cortical network sensitive to stimulus salience in a neutral behavioral context across multiple sensory modalities. Journal of Neurophysiology, 87, 615–620. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Fullam, R. S., McKie, S., Dolan, M. C. (2009). Psychopathic traits and deception: Functional magnetic resonance imaging study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 194, 229–235. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Furedy, J. J. (1986). Lie detection as psychophysiological differentiation: Some fine lines. In M. Coles, E. Donchin, S. Porges (Eds.), Psychophysiology: Systems, processes, and applications – A handbook (pp. 683–700). New York, NY: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Furedy, J. J., Davis, C., Gurevich, M. (1988). Differentiation of deception as a psychological process: A psychophysiological approach. Psychophysiology, 25, 683–688. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Furedy, J. J., Gigliotti, F., Ben-Shakhar, G. (1994). Electrodermal differentiation of deception: The effect of choice versus no choice of deceptive items. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 18, 13–22. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gamer, M. (2010). Does the Guilty Actions Test allow for differentiating guilty participants from informed innocents? A re-examination. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 76, 19–24. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.01.009 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gamer, M. (2011). Detecting of deception and concealed information using neuroimaging techniques. In B. Verschuere, G. Ben-Shakhar, E. H. Meijer (Eds.), Memory detection: Theory and application of the Concealed Information Test (pp. 90–113). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gamer, M., Bauermann, T., Stoeter, P., Vossel, G. (2007). Covariations among fMRI, skin conductance, and behavioral data during processing of concealed information. Human Brain Mapping, 28, 1287–1301. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20343 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gamer, M., Klimecki, O., Bauermann, T., Stoeter, P., Vossel, G. (2012). fMRI-activation patterns in the detection of concealed information rely on memory-related effects. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7, 506–515. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsp005 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gamer, M., Verschuere, B., Crombez, G., Vossel, G. (2008). Combining physiological measures in the detection of concealed information. Physiology & Behavior, 95, 333–340. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.06.011 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ganis, G., Kosslyn, S. M., Stose, S., Thompson, W. L., Yurgelun-Todd, D. A. (2003). Neural correlates of different types of deception: An fMRI investigation. Cerebral Cortex, 13, 830–836. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ganis, G., Rosenfeld, J. P., Meixner, J., Kievit, R. A., Schendan, H. E. (2011). Lying in the scanner: Covert countermeasures disrupt deception detection by functional magnetic resonance imaging. NeuroImage, 55, 312–319. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.025 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gödert, H. W., Rill, H. G., Vossel, G. (2001). Psychophysiological differentiation of deception: The effects of electrodermal lability and mode of responding on skin conductance and heart rate. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 40, 61–75. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greely, H. T., Illes, J. (2007). Neuroscience-based lie detection: The urgent need for regulation. American Journal of Law & Medicine, 33, 377–431. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Haynes, J.-D., Rees, G. (2006). Decoding mental states from brain activity in humans. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7, 523–534. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Honts, C. R. (2004). The psychophysiological detection of deception. In P. A. Granhag, L. A. Strömwall (Eds.), The detection of deception in forensic contexts (pp. 103–123). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Honts, C. R., Amato, S. (2002). Countermeasures. In M. Kleiner (Ed.), Handbook of polygraph testing (pp. 251–264). New York, NY: Academic Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Honts, C. R., Devitt, M. K., Winbush, M., Kircher, J. C. (1996). Mental and physical countermeasures reduce the accuracy of the concealed knowledge test. Psychophysiology, 33, 84–92. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Horowitz, S. W., Kircher, J. C., Honts, C. R., Raskin, D. C. (1997). The role of comparison questions in physiological detection of deception. Psychophysiology, 34, 108–115. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hu, X., Chen, H., Fu, G. (2012). A repeated lie becomes a truth? The effect of intentional control and training on deception. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 488 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00488 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Iacono, W. G. (2008). Effective policing: Understanding how polygraph tests work and are used. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 35, 1295–1308. doi: 10.1177/0093854808321529 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ito, A., Abe, N., Fujii, T., Hayashi, A., Ueno, A., Mugikura, S., … Mori, E. (2012). The contribution of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex to the preparation for deception and truth-telling. Brain Research, 1464, 43–52. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.05.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ito, A., Abe, N., Fujii, T., Ueno, A., Koseki, Y., Hashimoto, R., et al. (2011). The role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in deception when remembering neutral and emotional events. Neuroscience Research, 69, 121–128. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2010.11.001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kim, H. (2011). Differential neural activity in the recognition of old versus new events: An activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 34, 814–836. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21474 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kozel, F. A., Johnson, K. A., Grenesko, E. L., Laken, S. J., Kose, S., Lu, X., … George, M. S. (2009). Functional MRI detection of deception after committing a mock sabotage crime. Journal of Forensic Sciences, 54, 220–231. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kozel, F. A., Johnson, K. A., Laken, S. J., Grenesko, E. L., Smith, J. A., Walker, J., George, M. S. (2009). Can simultaneously acquired electrodermal activity improve accuracy of fMRI detection of deception? Social Neuroscience, 4, 510–517. doi: 10.1080/17470910801907168 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kozel, F. A., Johnson, K. A., Mu, Q., Grenesko, E. L., Laken, S. J., George, M. S. (2005). Detecting deception using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Biological Psychiatry, 58, 605–613. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kozel, F. A., Laken, S. J., Johnson, K. A., Boren, B., Mapes, K. S., Morgan, P. S., George, M. S. (2009). Replication of functional MRI detection of deception. Open Forensic Science Journal, 2, 6–11. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kozel, F. A., Padgett, T. M., George, M. S. (2004). A replication study of the neural correlates of deception. Behavioral Neuroscience, 118, 852–856. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.118.4.852 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kozel, F. A., Revell, L. J., Lorberbaum, J. P., Shastri, A., Elhai, J. D., Horner, M. D., … George, M. S. (2004). A pilot study of functional magnetic resonance imaging brain correlates of deception in healthy young men. Journal of Neuropsychiatry & Clinical Neurosciences, 16, 295–305. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Langleben, D. D. (2008). Detection of deception with fMRI: Are we there yet? Legal and Criminological Psychology, 13, 1–9. doi: 10.1348/135532507X251641 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Langleben, D. D., Loughead, J. W., Bilker, W. B., Ruparel, K., Childress, A. R., Busch, S. I., Gur, R. C. (2005). Telling truth from lie in individual subjects with fast event-related fMRI. Human Brain Mapping, 26, 262–272. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Langleben, D. D., Schroeder, L., Maldjian, J. A., Gur, R. C., McDonald, S., Ragland, J. D., … Childress, A. R. (2002). Brain activity during simulated deception: An event-related functional magnetic resonance study. NeuroImage, 15, 727–732. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lee, T. M. C., Lee, T. M. Y., Raine, A., Chan, C. C. H. (2010). Lying about the valence of affective pictures: An fMRI study. PLoS One, 5, e12291 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0012291 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Lykken, D. T. (1959). The GSR in the detection of guilt. Journal of Applied Psychology, 43, 385–388. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lykken, D. T. (1974). Psychology and the lie detector industry. American Psychologist, 29, 725–739. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lykken, D. T. (1998). A tremor in the blood: Uses and abuses of the lie detector (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Plenum Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • MacLaren, V. V. (2001). A quantitative review of the Guilty Knowledge Test. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 674–683. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Marchewka, A., Jednorog, K., Falkiewicz, M., Szeszkowski, W., Grabowska, A., Szatkowska, I. (2012). Sex, lies and fMRI – gender differences in neural basis of deception. PLoS One, 7, e43076 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0043076 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Marston, M. W. (1938). The lie detector test. New York, NY: Smith. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Matte, J. A. (1996). Forensic psychophysiology using the polygraph: Scientific truth verification – lie detection. Williamsville, NY: J. A. M.. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • McCabe, D. (2011). The influence of fMRI lie detection evidence on juror decision‐making. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 577, 566–577. doi: 10.1002/bsl First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Monteleone, G. T., Phan, K. L., Nusbaum, H. C., Fitzgerald, D., Irick, J.-S. (2009). Detection of deception using fMRI: Better than chance, but well below perfection. Social Neuroscience, 4, 528–538. doi: 10.1080/17470910801903530 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nakayama, M. (2002). Practical use of the concealed information test for criminal investigation in Japan. In M. Kleiner (Ed.), Handbook of polygraph testing (pp. 49–86). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • National Research Council . (2003). The polygraph and lie detection. Washington, DC: National Academies Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Nose, I., Murai, J., Taira, M. (2009). Disclosing concealed information on the basis of cortical activations. NeuroImage, 44, 1380–1386. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nuñez, J. M., Casey, B. J., Egner, T., Hare, T., Hirsch, J. (2005). Intentional false responding shares neural substrates with response conflict and cognitive control. NeuroImage, 25, 267–277. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.10.041 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Patrick, C. J., Iacono, W. G. (1989). Psychopathy, threat, and polygraph test accuracy. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 347–355. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Phan, K. L., Magalhaes, A., Ziemlewicz, T. J., Fitzgerald, D. A., Green, C., Smith, W. (2005). Neural correlates of telling lies: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study at 4 tesla. Academic Radiology, 12, 164–172. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Raskin, D., Honts, C. R. (2002). The comparison question test. In M. Kleiner (Ed.), Handbook of polygraph testing (pp. 1–47). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Reid, J. E. (1947). A revised questioning technique in lie detection tests. Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 37, 542–547. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ridderinkhof, K. R., Ullsperger, M., Crone, E. A., Nieuwenhuis, S. (2004). The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science, 306, 443–447. doi: 10.1126/science.1100301 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rissman, J., Greely, H. T., Wagner, A. D. (2010). Detecting individual memories through the neural decoding of memory states and past experience. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107, 9849–9854. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Rottschy, C., Langner, R., Dogan, I., Reetz, K., Laird, A. R., Schulz, J. B., … Eickhoff, S. B. (2012). Modelling neural correlates of working memory: A coordinate-based meta-analysis. NeuroImage, 60, 830–846. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.050 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rusconi, E., Mitchener-Nissen, T. (2013). Prospects of functional magnetic resonance imaging as lie detector. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 594 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00594 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schauer, F. (2010). Neuroscience, lie-detection, and the law: Contrary to the prevailing view, the suitability of brain-based lie-detection for courtroom or forensic use should be determined according to legal and not scientific standards. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 101–103. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Simpson, J. R. (2008). Functional MRI lie detection: Too good to be true? Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 36, 491–498. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Sip, K. E., Carmel, D., Marchant, J. L., Li, J., Petrovic, P., Roepstorff, A., … Frith, C. D. (2013). When Pinocchio’s nose does not grow: Belief regarding lie-detectability modulates production of deception. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 1–11. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00016 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sip, K. E., Lynge, M., Wallentin, M., McGregor, W. B., Frith, C. D., Roepstorff, A. (2010). The production and detection of deception in an interactive game. Neuropsychologia, 48, 3619–3626. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.08.013 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sip, K. E., Roepstorff, A., McGregor, W., Frith, C. D. (2008). Detecting deception: The scope and limits. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 48–53. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.11.008 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Spence, S. A., Farrow, T. F., Herford, A. E., Wilkinson, I. D., Zheng, Y., Woodruff, P. W. (2001). Behavioural and functional anatomical correlates of deception in humans. Neuroreport, 12, 2849–2853. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Spence, S. A., Kaylor-Hughes, C. J., Brook, M. L., Lankappa, S. T., Wilkinson, I. D. (2008). “Munchausen’s syndrome by proxy” or a “miscarriage of justice”? An initial application of functional neuroimaging to the question of guilt versus innocence. European Psychiatry, 23, 309–314. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Spence, S. A., Kaylor-Hughes, C., Farrow, T. F. D., Wilkinson, I. D. (2008). Speaking of secrets and lies: The contribution of ventrolateral prefrontal cortex to vocal deception. NeuroImage, 40, 1411–1418. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tian, F., Sharma, V., Kozel, F. A., Liu, H. (2009). Functional near-infrared spectroscopy to investigate hemodynamic responses to deception in the prefrontal cortex. Brain Research, 1303, 120–130. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • United States v. Semrau . No. 07e10074 M1/P, Report and Recommendation (W.D. Tenn. May 31, 2010. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Van Bockstaele, B., Verschuere, B., Moens, T., Suchotzki, K., Debey, E., Spruyt, A. (2012). Learning to lie: Effects of practice on the cognitive cost of lying. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 1–8. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00526 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Verschuere, B., Spruyt, A., Meijer, E. H., Otgaar, H. (2011). The ease of lying. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 908–911. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.10.023 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vrij, A. (2008). Detecting lies and deceit. Pitfalls and opportunities. Chichester, UK: Wiley. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Wilson v. Corestaff Services L.P. , 2010 NY Slip Op 20176, 1 (N.Y. Sup. Ct. 2010); 900 N.Y.S.2d 639; 2010 N.Y. Misc. LEXIS 1044 (2010). First citation in articleGoogle Scholar