Skip to main content
Research Article

Irrelevant Speech Disrupts Item-Context Binding

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000212

The present study examines the effects of irrelevant speech on immediate memory. Previous research led to the suggestion that auditory distractors particularly impair memory for serial order. These findings were explained by assuming that irrelevant speech disrupts the formation and maintenance of links between adjacent items in a to-be-remembered sequence, resulting in a loss of order information. Here we propose a more general explanation of these findings by claiming that the capacity to form and maintain item-context bindings is generally impaired by the presence of auditory distractors. The results of Experiment 1 show that memory for the association between an item and its background color is drastically impaired by irrelevant speech, just as memory for the association between an item and its serial position. In Experiment 2 it was examined whether the disrupting effects of irrelevant sound are limited to memory for item-context associations or whether item memory is also affected by the auditory distractors. The results revealed that irrelevant speech disrupts both item memory and item-context binding. The results suggest that the effects of irrelevant sound on immediate memory are more general than previously assumed, which has important theoretical and applied implications.

References

  • Baayen, R. H. , Piepenbrock, R. , van Rijn, H. (1993). The CELEX lexical database (Release 1) [CD-ROM]. Philadelphia, PA: Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Beaman, C. P. (2005). Irrelevant sound effects amongst younger and older adults: Objective findings and subjective insights. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 17, 241–265. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Beaman, C. P. , Jones, D. M. (1997). Role of serial order in the irrelevant speech effect: Tests of the changing-state hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 459–471. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Beaman, C. P. , Jones, D. M. (1998). Irrelevant sound disrupts order information in free recall as in serial recall. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 81, 615–636. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Beaman, C. P. , Neath, I. , Surprenant, A. M. (2008). Modeling distributions of immediate memory effects: No strategies needed? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 34, 219–229. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Beaman, C. P. , Röer, J. P. (2009). Learning and failing to learn within immediate memory. In N. Taatgen, H. van Rijn (Eds.), Proceedings of the 31st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bell, R. , Buchner, A. (2007). Equivalent irrelevant-sound effects for old and young adults. Memory & Cognition, 35, 352–364. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Bell, R. , Dentale, S. , Buchner, A. , Mayr, S. (2010). ERP correlates of the irrelevant sound effect. Psychophysiology, 47, 1182–1191. First citation in articleMedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Bell, R. , Röer, J. P. , Dentale, S. , Buchner, A. (2012). Habituation of the irrelevant sound effect: Evidence for an attentional theory of short-term memory disruption. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 1542–1557. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Brown, L. A. , Brockmole, J. R. (2010). The role of attention in binding visual features in working memory: Evidence from cognitive ageing. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 2067–2079. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brown, E. , Deffenbacher, K. , Sturgill, W. (1977). Memory for faces and the circumstances of encounter. Journal of Applied Psychology, 62, 311–318. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Buchner, A. , Bell, R. , Rothermund, K. , Wentura, D. (2008). Sound source location modulates the irrelevant sound effect. Memory & Cognition, 36, 617–628. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Campbell, T. , Beaman, C. P. , Berry, D. C. (2002). Auditory memory and the irrelevant sound effect: Further evidence for changing-state disruption. Memory, 10, 199–214. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Campbell, T. , Winkler, I. , Kujala, T. (2007). N1 and the mismatch negativity are spatiotemporally distinct ERP components: Disruption of immediate memory by auditory distraction can be related to N1. Psychophysiology, 44, 530–540. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Campbell, T. , Winkler, I. , Kujala, T. , Näätänen, R. (2003). The N1 hypothesis and irrelevant sound: Evidence from token set size effects. Cognitive Brain Research, 18, 39–47. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Chein, J. M. , Fiez, J. A. (2010). Evaluating models of working memory through the effects of concurrent irrelevant information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 159, 117–137. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Colle, H. A. , Welsh, A. (1976). Acoustic masking in primary memory. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 15, 17–31. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cowan, N. (1995). Attention and memory: An integrated framework. London, UK: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Elsley, J. V. , Parmentier, F. B. R. (2009). Is verbal-spatial binding in working memory impaired by a concurrent memory load? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 1696–1705. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Escera, C. , Alho, K. , Schröger, E. , Winkler, I. (2000). Involuntary attention and distractibility as evaluated with event-related brain potentials. Audiology and Neuro-Otology, 5, 151–166. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Gisselgard, J. , Udden, J. , Ingvar, M. , Petersson, K. M. (2007). Disruption of order information by irrelevant items: A serial recognition paradigm. Acta Psychologica, 124, 356–369. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Hadlington, L. , Bridges, A. M. , Darby, R. J. (2004). Auditory location in the irrelevant sound effect: The effects of presenting auditory stimuli to either the left ear, right ear or both ears. Brain and Cognition, 55, 545–557. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Hughes, R. W. , Vachon, F. , Jones, D. M. (2005). Auditory attentional capture during serial recall: Violations at encoding of an algorithm-based neural model? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 31, 736–749. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Hughes, R. W. , Vachon, F. , Jones, D. M. (2007). Disruption of short-term memory by changing and deviant sounds: Support for a duplex-mechanism account of auditory distraction. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 1050–1061. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Jones, D. M. (1993). Objects, streams, and threads of auditory attention. In L. Weiskrantz, A. D. Baddeley (Eds.), Attention: Selection, awareness, and control: A tribute to Donald Broadbent (pp. 87–104). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Jones, D. M. , Farrand, P. , Stuart, G. , Morris, N. (1995). Functional equivalence of verbal and spatial information in serial short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 1008–1018. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Jones, D. M. , Macken, W. J. (1993). Irrelevant tones produce an irrelevant speech effect: Implications for phonological coding in working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 369–381. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jones, D. M. , Macken, W. J. , Murray, A. C. (1993). Disruption of visual short-term memory by changing-state auditory stimuli: The role of segmentation. Memory & Cognition, 21, 318–328. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Jones, D. M. , Madden, C. , Miles, C. (1992). Privileged access by irrelevant speech to short-term memory: The role of changing state. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 44, 645–669. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Klatte, M. , Lachmann, T. , Schlittmeier, S. , Hellbruck, J. (2010). The irrelevant sound effect in short-term memory: Is there developmental change? European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 22, 1168–1191. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • LeCompte, D. C. (1994). Extending the irrelevant speech effect beyond serial recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 1396–1408. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Little, J. S. , Martin, F. H. , Thomson, R. H. S. (2010). Speech versus non-speech as irrelevant sound: Controlling acoustic variation. Biological Psychology, 85, 62–70. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Meiser, T. , Bröder, A. (2002). Memory for multidimensional source information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 116–137. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Meiser, T. , Sattler, C. , Weisser, K. (2008). Binding of multidimensional context information as a distinctive characteristic of remember judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 54, 32–49. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Näätänen, R. (1990). The role of attention in auditory information processing as revealed by event-related potentials and other brain measures of cognitive function. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 15, 201–288. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Oberauer, K. (2005). Binding and inhibition in working memory: Individual and age differences in short-term recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 154, 368–387. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Perfect, T. J. , Mayes, A. R. , Downes, J. J. , Van Eijk, R. (1996). Does context discriminate recollection from familiarity in recognition memory? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 49, 797–813. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Röer, J. P. , Bell, R. , Dentale, S. , Buchner, A. (2011). The role of habituation and attentional orienting in the disruption of short-term memory performance. Memory & Cognition, 59, 839–850. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schlittmeier, S. J. , Weisz, N. , Bertrand, O. (2011). What characterizes changing-state speech in affecting short-term memory? An EEG study on the irrelevant sound effect. Psychophysiology, 48, 1669–1680. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Stokes, K. A. , Arnell, K. M. (2012). New considerations for the cognitive locus of impairment in the irrelevant-sound effect. Memory & Cognition, 40, 918–931. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Treisman, A. M. (2003). Consciousness and perceptual binding. The unity of consciousness: Binding, integration, and dissociation (pp. 95–113). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Treisman, A. M. , Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97–136. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Tremblay, S. , Macken, W. J. , Jones, D. M. (2000). Elimination of the word length effect by irrelevant sound revisited. Memory & Cognition, 28, 841–846. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Tremblay, S. , Nicholls, A. P. , Alford, D. , Jones, D. M. (2000). The irrelevant sound effect: Does speech play a special role? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 26, 1750–1754. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Tremblay, S. , Parmentier, F. B. R. , Hodgetts, H. , Hughes, R. W. , Jones, D. M. (2012). Disruption of verbal-spatial serial memory by extraneous air-traffic speech. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 1, 73–79. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Troyer, A. K. , Winocur, G. , Craik, F. I. M. , Moscovitch, M. (1999). Source memory and divided attention: Reciprocal costs to primary and secondary tasks. Neuropsychology, 13, 467–474. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Weisz, N. , Schlittmeier, S. J. (2006). Detrimental effects of irrelevant speech on serial recall of visual items are reflected in reduced visual N1 and reduced theta activity. Cerebral Cortex, 16, 1097–1105. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Wheeler, M. E. , Treisman, A. M. (2002). Binding in short-term visual memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131, 48–64. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar