Irrelevant Speech Disrupts Item-Context Binding
Abstract
The present study examines the effects of irrelevant speech on immediate memory. Previous research led to the suggestion that auditory distractors particularly impair memory for serial order. These findings were explained by assuming that irrelevant speech disrupts the formation and maintenance of links between adjacent items in a to-be-remembered sequence, resulting in a loss of order information. Here we propose a more general explanation of these findings by claiming that the capacity to form and maintain item-context bindings is generally impaired by the presence of auditory distractors. The results of Experiment 1 show that memory for the association between an item and its background color is drastically impaired by irrelevant speech, just as memory for the association between an item and its serial position. In Experiment 2 it was examined whether the disrupting effects of irrelevant sound are limited to memory for item-context associations or whether item memory is also affected by the auditory distractors. The results revealed that irrelevant speech disrupts both item memory and item-context binding. The results suggest that the effects of irrelevant sound on immediate memory are more general than previously assumed, which has important theoretical and applied implications.
References
1993). The CELEX lexical database (Release 1) [CD-ROM]. Philadelphia, PA: Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania.
(2005). Irrelevant sound effects amongst younger and older adults: Objective findings and subjective insights. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 17, 241–265.
(1997). Role of serial order in the irrelevant speech effect: Tests of the changing-state hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 459–471.
(1998). Irrelevant sound disrupts order information in free recall as in serial recall. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 81, 615–636.
(2008). Modeling distributions of immediate memory effects: No strategies needed? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 34, 219–229.
(2009). Learning and failing to learn within immediate memory. In , Proceedings of the 31st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
(2007). Equivalent irrelevant-sound effects for old and young adults. Memory & Cognition, 35, 352–364.
(2010). ERP correlates of the irrelevant sound effect. Psychophysiology, 47, 1182–1191.
(2012). Habituation of the irrelevant sound effect: Evidence for an attentional theory of short-term memory disruption. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 1542–1557.
(2010). The role of attention in binding visual features in working memory: Evidence from cognitive ageing. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 2067–2079.
(1977). Memory for faces and the circumstances of encounter. Journal of Applied Psychology, 62, 311–318.
(2008). Sound source location modulates the irrelevant sound effect. Memory & Cognition, 36, 617–628.
(2002). Auditory memory and the irrelevant sound effect: Further evidence for changing-state disruption. Memory, 10, 199–214.
(2007). N1 and the mismatch negativity are spatiotemporally distinct ERP components: Disruption of immediate memory by auditory distraction can be related to N1. Psychophysiology, 44, 530–540.
(2003). The N1 hypothesis and irrelevant sound: Evidence from token set size effects. Cognitive Brain Research, 18, 39–47.
(2010). Evaluating models of working memory through the effects of concurrent irrelevant information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 159, 117–137.
(1976). Acoustic masking in primary memory. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 15, 17–31.
(1995). Attention and memory: An integrated framework. London, UK: Oxford University Press.
(2009). Is verbal-spatial binding in working memory impaired by a concurrent memory load? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 1696–1705.
(2000). Involuntary attention and distractibility as evaluated with event-related brain potentials. Audiology and Neuro-Otology, 5, 151–166.
(2007). Disruption of order information by irrelevant items: A serial recognition paradigm. Acta Psychologica, 124, 356–369.
(2004). Auditory location in the irrelevant sound effect: The effects of presenting auditory stimuli to either the left ear, right ear or both ears. Brain and Cognition, 55, 545–557.
(2005). Auditory attentional capture during serial recall: Violations at encoding of an algorithm-based neural model? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 31, 736–749.
(2007). Disruption of short-term memory by changing and deviant sounds: Support for a duplex-mechanism account of auditory distraction. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 1050–1061.
(1993). Objects, streams, and threads of auditory attention. In , Attention: Selection, awareness, and control: A tribute to Donald Broadbent (pp. 87–104). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
(1995). Functional equivalence of verbal and spatial information in serial short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 1008–1018.
(1993). Irrelevant tones produce an irrelevant speech effect: Implications for phonological coding in working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 369–381.
(1993). Disruption of visual short-term memory by changing-state auditory stimuli: The role of segmentation. Memory & Cognition, 21, 318–328.
(1992). Privileged access by irrelevant speech to short-term memory: The role of changing state. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 44, 645–669.
(2010). The irrelevant sound effect in short-term memory: Is there developmental change? European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 22, 1168–1191.
(1994). Extending the irrelevant speech effect beyond serial recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 1396–1408.
(2010). Speech versus non-speech as irrelevant sound: Controlling acoustic variation. Biological Psychology, 85, 62–70.
(2002). Memory for multidimensional source information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 116–137.
(2008). Binding of multidimensional context information as a distinctive characteristic of remember judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 54, 32–49.
(1990). The role of attention in auditory information processing as revealed by event-related potentials and other brain measures of cognitive function. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 15, 201–288.
(2005). Binding and inhibition in working memory: Individual and age differences in short-term recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 154, 368–387.
(1996). Does context discriminate recollection from familiarity in recognition memory? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 49, 797–813.
(2011). The role of habituation and attentional orienting in the disruption of short-term memory performance. Memory & Cognition, 59, 839–850.
(2011). What characterizes changing-state speech in affecting short-term memory? An EEG study on the irrelevant sound effect. Psychophysiology, 48, 1669–1680.
(2012). New considerations for the cognitive locus of impairment in the irrelevant-sound effect. Memory & Cognition, 40, 918–931.
(2003). Consciousness and perceptual binding. The unity of consciousness: Binding, integration, and dissociation (pp. 95–113). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
(1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97–136.
(2000). Elimination of the word length effect by irrelevant sound revisited. Memory & Cognition, 28, 841–846.
(2000). The irrelevant sound effect: Does speech play a special role? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 26, 1750–1754.
(2012). Disruption of verbal-spatial serial memory by extraneous air-traffic speech. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 1, 73–79.
(1999). Source memory and divided attention: Reciprocal costs to primary and secondary tasks. Neuropsychology, 13, 467–474.
(2006). Detrimental effects of irrelevant speech on serial recall of visual items are reflected in reduced visual N1 and reduced theta activity. Cerebral Cortex, 16, 1097–1105.
(2002). Binding in short-term visual memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131, 48–64.
(