Skip to main content
Research Article

At the Boundaries of Misattribution

Does Positivity Influence Judgments of Familiarity in the Affect Misattribution Procedure?

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000379

Abstract. Priming effects in the Affect Misattribution Procedure (AMP) have been explained by a misattribution of prime-related affect to neutral targets. However, the measure has been criticized for being susceptible to intentional use of prime-features in judgments of the targets. To isolate the contribution of unintentional processes, the present research expanded on the finding that positive affect can be misattributed to familiarity (i.e., positivity-familiarity effect). To the extent that prime-valence is deemed irrelevant for judgments of target-familiarity, positivity-familiarity effects in the AMP could potentially rule out intentional use of the primes. Seven experiments collectively suggest that prime-valence influences judgments of target-familiarity in the AMP, but only when the task context does not suggest a normatively accurate response to the familiarity-judgment task. Relations of positivity-familiarity effects to self-reported use of prime-valence revealed mixed results regarding the role of intentional processes. Implications for the AMP and misattribution effects are discussed.

References

  • Bar-Anan, Y. & Nosek, B. A. (2012). Reporting intentional rating of the primes predicts priming effects in the affective misattribution procedure. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 1194–1208. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167212446835 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Blaison, C., Imhoff, R., Hess, U. & Banse, R. (2012). The affect misattribution procedure: Hot or not? Emotion, 12, 403–412. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026907 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Brown, A. S. & Marsh, E. J. (2009). Creating illusions of past encounter through brief exposure. Psychological Science, 20, 534–538. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02337.x First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T. & Gosling, S. D. (2011). Amazon’s mechanical turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 3–5. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691610393980 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Cameron, C. D., Brown-Iannuzzi, J. & Payne, B. K. (2012). Sequential priming measures of implicit social cognition: A meta-analysis of associations with behaviors and explicit attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 16, 330–350. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868312440047 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Corneille, O., Monin, B. & Pleyers, G. (2005). Is positivity a cue or a response option? Warm glow vs. evaluative matching in the familiarity for attractive and not-so-attractive faces. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 431–437. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2004.08.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • De Houwer, J. & Smith, C. T. (2013). Go with your gut! Effects in the affect misattribution procedure become stronger when participants are encouraged to rely on their gut feelings. Social Psychology, 44, 299–302. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000115 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • De Houwer, J., Teige-Mocigemba, S., Spruyt, A. & Moors, A. (2009). Implicit measures: A normative analysis and review. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 347–368. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014211 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Deutsch, R. & Gawronski, B. (2009). When the method makes a difference: Antagonistic effects on “automatic evaluations” as a function of task characteristics of the measure. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 101–114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2008.09.001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Deutsch, R., Kordts-Freudinger, R., Gawronski, B. & Strack, F. (2009). Fast and fragile: A new look at the automaticity of negation processing. Experimental Psychology, 56, 434–446. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169.56.6.434 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Dougal, S. & Rotello, C. M. (2007). “Remembering” emotional words is based on response bias, not recollection. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 423–429. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194083 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Eder, A. B. & Deutsch, R. (2015). Watch the target! Effects in the affective misattribution procedure become weaker (but not eliminated) when participants are motivated to provide accurate responses to the target. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1442. https://doi.org/10.3389%2Ffpsyg.2015.01442 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G. & Buchner, A. (2007). G*power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193146 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Garcia-Marques, T., Mackie, D. M., Claypool, H. M. & Garcia-Marques, L. (2004). Positivity can cue familiarity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 585–593. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167203262856 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Gawronski, B., Cunningham, W. A., LeBel, E. P. & Deutsch, R. (2010). Attentional influences on affective priming: Does categorization influence spontaneous evaluations of multiply categorizable objects? Cognition and Emotion, 24, 1008–1025. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930903112712 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gawronski, B. & De Houwer, J. (2014). Implicit measures in social and personality psychology. In H. T. ReisC. M. JuddEds., Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology (2nd ed., pp. 283–310). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Gawronski, B. & Ye, Y. (2014). What drives priming effects in the affect misattribution procedure? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40, 3–15. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167213502548 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Gawronski, B. & Ye, Y. (2015). Prevention of intention invention in the affect misattribution procedure. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 6, 101–108. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550614543029 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Imhoff, R., Schmidt, A. F., Bernhardt, J., Dierksmeier, A. & Banse, R. (2011). An inkblot for sexual preference: A semantic variant of the affect misattribution procedure. Cognition and Emotion, 25, 676–690. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2010.508260 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M. & Cuthbert, B. N. (2008). The international affective picture system (IAPS): Technical manual and affective ratings. Gainesville, FL: The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Loersch, C. & Payne, B. K. (2011). The situated inference model: An integrative account of the effects of primes on perception, behavior, and motivation. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 234–252. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691611406921 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Monin, B. (2003). The warm glow heuristic: When liking leads to familiarity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 1035–1048. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.6.1035 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Murphy, S. T. & Zajonc, R. B. (1993). Affect, cognition, and awareness: Affective priming with optimal and suboptimal stimulus exposure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 723–739. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.64.5.723 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Nisbett, R. E. & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.84.3.231 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Oikawa, M., Aarts, H. & Oikawa, H. (2011). There is a fire burning in my heart: The role of causal attribution in affect transfer. Cognition and Emotion, 25, 156–163. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931003680061 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Olson, M. A. & Fazio, R. H. (2003). Relations between implicit measures of prejudice: What are we measuring? Psychological Science, 14, 636–639. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1477.x First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Paulhus, D. L. (1984). Two component models of social desirable responding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 598–609. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.46.3.598 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Payne, B. K., Brown-Iannuzzi, J. L., Burkley, M., Arbuckle, N. L., Cooley, E., Cameron, C. D. & Lundberg, K. B. (2013). Intention invention and the affect misattribution procedure: Reply to Bar-Anan and Nosek (2012). Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39, 375–386. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167212475225 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Payne, B. K., Cheng, S. M., Govorun, O. & Stewart, B. D. (2005). An inkblot for attitudes: Affect misattribution as implicit measurement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 277–293. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.89.3.277 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Payne, B. K., Hall, D., Cameron, C. D. & Bishara, A. J. (2010). A process model of affect misattribution. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 1397–1408. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167210383440 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Payne, B. K. & Lundberg, K. B. (2014). The affect misattribution procedure: Ten years of evidence on reliability, validity, and mechanisms. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 8, 672–686. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12148 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Phaf, R. H. & Rotteveel, M. (2005). Affective modulation of recognition bias. Emotion, 5, 309–318. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.5.3.309 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Phelps, E. A. & Sharot, T. (2008). How (and why) emotion enhances the subjective sense of recollection. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 147–152. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00565.x First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Sava, F. A., Maricutoiu, L. P., Rusu, S., Macsinga, I., Virga, D., Cheng, C. M. & Payne, B. K. (2012). An inkblot for the implicit assessment of personality: The semantic misattribution procedure. European Journal of Personality, 26, 613–628. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.1861 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Teige-Mocigemba, S., Becker, M., Sherman, J. W., Reichardt, R. & Klauer, K. C. (2017). The affect misattribution procedure: In search of prejudice effects. Experimental Psychology, 64, 215–230. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000364 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Teige-Mocigemba, S., Penzl, B., Becker, M., Henn, L. & Klauer, K. C. (2016). Controlling the “uncontrollable”: Faking effects on the affect misattribution procedure. Cognition and Emotion, 30, 1470–1484. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2015.1070793 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Weil, R. (2016). A look inside the toolbox: Measurement procedures in implicit social cognition. In F. T. L. LeongD. BartramF. M. CheungK. F. GeisingerD. IliescuEds., The ITC international handbook of testing and assessment (pp. 89–105). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Westerman, D. L., Lloyd, M. E. & Miller, J. K. (2002). The attribution of perceptual fluency in recognition memory: The role of expectation. Journal of Memory and Language, 47, 607–617. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(02)00022-0 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Winkielman, P., Schwarz, N., Fazendeiro, T. & Reber, R. (2003). The hedonic marking of processing fluency: Implications for evaluative judgment. In J. MuschK. C. KlauerEds., The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion (pp. 189–217). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9, 1–27. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0025848 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar