Skip to main content
Short Research Article

How Emotions Modulate Arithmetic Performance

A Study in Arithmetic Problem Verification Tasks

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000460

Abstract. The goal of the present study was to test whether and how emotions influence arithmetic performance. Participants had to verify arithmetic problems. True problems were either easier or harder problems. False problems were parity-match or parity-mismatch problems. The odd/even status of proposed and correct answers was the same in parity-match problems (e.g., 19 × 7 = 131) and different in parity-mismatch problems (e.g., 17 × 9 = 152). Before each problem, participants saw a positive (e.g., smiling baby), negative (e.g., mutilations), or neutral pictures (e.g., neutral face) selected from International Affective Picture System (IAPS). They had to decide whether each picture includes a person or not before verifying each arithmetic problem. Results showed different effects of emotion on true- and false problem verification. Participants’ performance on true problems showed decreased problem-difficulty after processing negative pictures and increased difficulty effects after processing positive pictures. On false problems, we found smaller parity-violation effects after negative pictures (i.e., decreased performance on parity-mismatch problems), together with larger parity-violation effects after positive pictures (i.e., decreased performance on parity-match problems). These findings suggest that emotions influence arithmetic performance via which strategy is used and how each strategy is executed on each problem. They have important implications for understanding the role of emotions on arithmetic performance, and more generally on how emotions influence cognition.

References

  • Anders, R., Hinault, T., & Lemaire, P. (2018). Heuristics versus direct calculation, and age-related differences in multiplication: An evidence accumulation account of plausibility decisions in arithmetic. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 30, 18–34. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2017.1397676 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ashcraft, M. H., & Kirk, E. P. (2001). The relationships among working memory, math anxiety, and performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 224–237. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.130.2.224 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Ashcraft, M. H., & Rudig, N. O. (2012). Higher cognition is altered by non-cognitive factors: How affect enhances and disrupts mathematics performance in adolescence and young adulthood. In V. F. ReynaS. B. ChapmanM. R. DoughertyJ. ConfreyEds., The adolescent brain: Learning, reasoning, and decision making (pp. 243–263). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/13493-009 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Beilock, S. L., & Carr, T. H. (2005). When high-powered people fail: Working memory and “choking under pressure” in math. Psychological Science, 16, 101–105. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00789 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Blanchette, I., & Campbell, M. (2012). Reasoning about highly emotional topics: Syllogistic reasoning in a group of war veterans. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 24, 157–164. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2011.603693 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Blanchette, I., & Caparos, S. (2013). When emotions improve reasoning: The possible roles of relevance and utility. Thinking & Reasoning, 19, 399–413. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2013.791642 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Blanchette, I., & Richards, A. (2004). Reasoning about emotional and neutral materials. Is logic affected by emotion? Psychological Science, 15, 745–752. https://doi.org/10.1111.j.0956-7976.2004.0751 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Blanchette, I., Richards, A., Melnyk, L., & Lavda, A. (2007). Reasoning about emotional contents following shocking terrorist attacks: A tale of three cities. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 13, 47–56. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-898x.13.1.47 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Carey, E., Hill, F., Devine, A., & Szucs, D. (2015). The chicken or the egg? The direction of the relationship between mathematics anxiety and mathematics performance. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1987. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01987 First citation in articleMedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Carretié, L. (2014). Exogenous (automatic) attention to emotional stimuli: A review. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 14, 1228–1258. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-014-0270-2 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Cohen-Kadosh, R. C.Dowker, A. (Eds.). (2015). The Oxford handbook of numerical cognition. Oxford, UK: Oxford Library of Psychology. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • DeCaro, M. S., Rotar, K. E., Kendra, M. S., & Beilock, S. L. (2010). Diagnosing and alleviating the impact of performance pressure on mathematical problem solving. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 1619–1630. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210903474286 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dowker, A., Sarkar, A., & Looi, C. Y. (2016). Mathematics anxiety: What have we learned in 60 years? Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 508. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00508 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Edelstein, R. S., & Levine, L. J. (2010). Emotion and memory narrowing: A review and goal-relevance approach. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 833–875. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930902738863 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Figueira, J. S. B., Oliveira, L., Pereira, M. G., Pacheco, L. B., Lobo, I., Motta-Ribeiro, G. C., & David, I. A. (2017). An unpleasant emotional state reduces working memory capacity: Electrophysiological evidence. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12, 984–992. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsx030 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Gilmore, C., Göbel, S. M., & Inglis, M. (2018). An introduction to mathematical cognition. London, UK: Routledge. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hembree, R. (1990). The nature, effects, and relief of mathematics anxiety. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 21, 33–46. https://doi.org/10.2307/749455 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kleinsorge, T. (2007). Anticipatory modulation of interference induced by unpleasant pictures. Cognition and Emotion, 21, 404–421. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930600625032 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kleinsorge, T. (2009). Anticipation selectively enhances interference exerted by pictures of negative valence. Experimental Psychology, 56, 228–235. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169.56.4.228 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Krueger, L. E. (1986). Why 2 × 2 = 5 looks so wrong: On the odd–even rule in product verification. Memory & Cognition, 14, 141–149. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Krueger, L. E., & Hallford, E. W. (1984). Why 2 + 2 = 5 looks so wrong: On the odd–even rule in sum verification. Memory & Cognition, 12, 171–180. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (2007). The International Affective Picture System (IAPS) in the study of emotion and attention. In J. A. CoanJ. J. B. AllenEds., Series in affective science. Handbook of emotion elicitation and assessment (pp. 29–46). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2008). International affective picture system (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual (Technical Report A-8). Gainesville, FL: University of Florida. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Lemaire, P., & Fayol, M. (1995). When plausibility judgments supersede fact retrieval: The example of the odd-even effect on product verification. Memory & Cognition, 23, 34–48. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03210555 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Lemaire, P., & Reder, L. (1999). What affects strategy selection in arithmetic? The example of parity and five effects on product verification. Memory & Cognition, 27, 364–382. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03211420 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Lochy, A., Seron, X., Delazer, M., & Butterworth, B. (2000). The odd-even effect in multiplication: Parity rule or familiarity with even numbers? Memory & Cognition, 28, 358–365. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03198551 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Richardson, F. C., & Suinn, R. M. (1972). The mathematics anxiety rating scale: Psychometric data. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 19, 551. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0033456 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Robinson, M. D.Watkins, E. R.Harmon-Jones, E. (Eds.). (2013). Handbook of cognition and emotion. New York, NY: Guilford Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Rowe, G., Hirsh, J. B., & Anderson, A. K. (2007). Positive affect increases the breadth of attentional selection. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 383–388. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0605198104 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Schimmack, U., & Derryberry, D. E. (2005). Attentional interference effects of emotional pictures: threat, negativity, or arousal? Emotion, 5, 55. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.5.1.55 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Siegler, R. S., & Lemaire, P. (1997). Older and younger adults’ strategy choices in multiplication: Testing predictions of ASCM using the choice/no-choice method. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126, 71. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.126.1.71 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Waring, J. D., & Kensinger, E. A. (2009). Effects of emotional valence and arousal upon memory trade-offs with aging. Psychology and Aging, 24, 412. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015526 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Xolocotzin, U. (Ed.). (2017). Understanding emotions in mathematical thinking and learning. London, UK: Academic Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Young, C. B., Wu, S. S., & Menon, V. (2012). The neurodevelopmental basis of math anxiety. Psychological Science, 23, 492–501. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611429134 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar