Skip to main content
Research Report

Understanding the Experience of Imaginary Audience in a Social Media Environment

Implications for Adolescent Development

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-1105/a000124

Adolescent use of social networking sites has grown quickly over the past decade. Despite this high level of use, less research has examined the interaction between adolescent development and use of social networking sites. Thus, the present study seeks to understand the relationship between adolescents’ Facebook use and their experience of a developmental construct, Imaginary Audience. Using survey data collected from 260 participants between the ages of 9 and 26, results suggested a positive relationship between Facebook use and Imaginary Audience ideation. Furthermore, to better understand this relationship, behavioral rehearsal was tested as a mediating variable. Results were supportive of this mediator. The present study was limited in two main ways: First, given the cross-sectional survey design, we were unable to make causal claims from our data. Second, the sample was predominantly homogenous in nature. Despite these limitations, however, the implications of these findings are twofold: First, this research indicates that Facebook use is related to characteristics of adolescent development. Future research should continue to examine this relationship pattern. Second, by connecting the study of media behaviors and developmental theory, we call attention to a timely and fruitful avenue for the interdisciplinary study of developmental phenomena and media.

References

  • Antheunis, M. L. , & Schouten, A. P. (2011). The effects of other-generated and system generated cues on adolescents’ perceived attractiveness on social networking sites. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 16(3), 391–406. doi: 10.1111/j.10836101.2011.01545.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Burke, M. , Marlow, C. , & Lento, T. (2009). Feed me: Motivating newcomer contribution in social network sites. In Proceedings of the 27th Annual SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (pp. 945–954). New York: ACM. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Elkind, D. (1967). Egocentrism in adolescence. Child Development, 38(4), 1025–1034. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fenigstein, A. , Scheier, M. F. , & Buss, A. H. (1975). Public and private self-consciousness: Assessment and theory. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 43(4), 522–527. doi: 10.1037/h0076760 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fogg, B. J. (2008). Mass interpersonal persuasion: An early view of a new phenomenon. In H. Oinas-Kukkonen, P. Hasle, M. Harjumaa, K. Segerståhl, & P. Øhrstrøm (Eds.), Proceedings of the Third International Conference on Persuasive Technology (pp. 35–46). Berlin: Springer. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lampe, C. , Ellison, N. B. , & Steinfield, C. (2008). Changes in use and perception of Facebook. In Proceedings of the 2008 ACM Conference on Computer-Supported Cooperative Work (pp. 721–730). New York: ACM doi: 10.1145/1460563.1460675 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lapsley, D. K. , FitzGerald, D. P. , Rice, K. G. , & Jackson, S. (1989). Separation individuation and the “new look” at the imaginary audience and personal fable: A test of an integrative model. Journal of Adolescent Research, 4(4), 483–505. doi: 10.1177/074355488944006 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lapsley, D. K. , & Rice, K. G. (1988). The “new look” at the imaginary audience and personal fable: Toward a general model of adolescent ego development. In D. K. Lapsley, & F. C. Power (Eds.), Self, ego, and identity: Integrative approaches (pp. 109–129). New York: Springer. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Litt, E. (2012). Knock, Knock. Who’s there? The Imagined Audience. Journal of Electronic and Broadcasting Media, 56(3), 330–345. doi: 10.1080/08838151.2012.705195 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McLaughlin, C. , & Vitak, J. (2012). Norm evolution and violation on Facebook. New Media & Society, 14(2), 299–315. doi: 10.1177/1461444811412712 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Moreno, M. (2010). Social networking sites and adolescents. Pediatric Annals, 39, 565–568. doi: 10.3928/00904481-20100825-07 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Moreno, M. A. , Briner, L. R. , Williams, A. , Walker, L. , & Christakis, D. A. (2009). Real use o “real cool”: Adolescents speak out about displayed alcohol references on social networking websites. Journal of Adolescent Health, 45, 420–422. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.04.015 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Preacher, K. J. , & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879–891. doi: 10.3758/BRM.40.3.879 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Remondet, J. H. , Hansson, R. O. , Rule, B. , & Winfrey, G. (1987). Rehearsal for widowhood. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 5(3), 285–297. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Slater, M. D. , & Hayes, A. F. (2010). The influence of youth music television viewership on changes in cigarette use and association with smoking peers: A social identity, reinforcing spiral perspective. Communication Research, 37(6), 751–773. doi: 10.1177/0093650210375953 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Slater, M. D. , Henry, K. L. , Swaim, R. C. , & Anderson, L. L. (2003). Violent media content and aggressiveness in adolescents: A downward spiral model. Communication Research, 30(6), 713–736. doi: 10.1177/0093650203258281 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Steinfield, C. , Ellison, N. B. , & Lampe, C. (2008). Social capital, self-esteem, and the use of online social networking sites: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 29(6), 434–445. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2008.07.002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Trepte, S. , & Reinecke, L. (2013). The reciprocal effects of social network site use and the disposition for self-disclosure: A longitudinal study. Computers in Human Behavior, 29(3), 1102–1112. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2012.10.002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Valkenburg, P. M. , & Peter, J. (2008). Adolescents’ identity experiments on the Internet: Consequences for social competence and self-concept unity. Communication Research, 35(2), 208–231. doi: 10.1177/0093650207313164 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Valkenburg, P. M. , & Peter, J. (2009). Social consequences of the Internet for adolescents: A decade of research. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(1), 1–5. doi: 0.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01595.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Valkenburg, P. M. , Peter, J. , & Schouten, A. P. (2006). Friend networking sites and their relationship to adolescents’ well-being and social self-esteem. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 9(5), 584–590. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2006.9.584 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Walrave, M. , Vanwesenbeeck, I. , & Heirman, W. (2012). Connecting and protecting? Comparing predictors of self-disclosure and privacy settings use between adolescents and adults. Cyberpsychology: Journal of Psychosocial Research on Cyberspace, 6(1). Article 3. doi: 10.5817/CP2012-1-3 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar