Skip to main content
Theoretical Article

The Media Psychology of Boredom and Mobile Media Use

Theoretical and Methodological Innovations

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-1105/a000340

Abstract. Boredom is a prevalent and relevant, yet understudied, negative emotion in the field of media psychology. This paper proposes novel theoretical foundations to study boredom as an emotion and its related regulation strategies in the context of mobile media. Due to their pervasive nature, mobile media allow for boredom regulation via passive and (inter)active exposure to a wide variety of media contents. It is still unclear how and through which processes mobile media provide successful boredom regulation. This paper first describes the existing scarce and mostly older literature on boredom from the field of media psychology and links this to recent insights from general psychology with as its core the meaning and attentional components (MAC) model (Westgate & Wilson, 2018). It then integrates media psychology predictions for mobile media into the MAC model and identifies gaps and opportunities to be tackled in future media psychology studies, by also taking into account the broader boredom findings from within general psychology, for example, those focusing on the meaning component. Finally, the paper provides a summary of the psychological and neurobiological mechanisms giving rise to boredom and proposes methodological innovations for studying the research questions that are still left unanswered. The aim is to inspire future media psychology research on boredom as a highly relevant emotional state and how boredom regulation through mobile media use for can be both a challenge and an opportunity for individuals’ well-being.

References

  • Acharya, U. R., Joseph, K. P., Kannathal, N., Lim, C. M., & Suri, J. S. (2006). Heart rate variability: A review. Medical and Biological Engineering and Computing, 44(12), 1031–1051. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11517-006-0119-0 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Agrawal, M., Mattar, M. G., Cohen, J. D., & Daw, N. D. (2021). The temporal dynamics of opportunity costs: A normative account of cognitive fatigue and boredom. Psychological Review. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000309 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Al-Saggaf, Y., MacCulloch, R., & Wiener, K. (2019). Trait boredom is a predictor of phubbing frequency. Journal of Technology in Behavioral Science, 4(3), 245–252. https://doi.org/10.1007/s41347-018-0080-4 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bench, S. W., & Lench, H. C. (2019). Boredom as a seeking state: Boredom prompts the pursuit of novel (even negative) experiences. Emotion, 19(2), 242–254. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000433 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Beyens, I., Pouwels, J. L., van Driel, I. I., Keijsers, L., & Valkenburg, P. M. (2020). The effect of social media on well-being differs from adolescent to adolescent. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-67727-7 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bieleke, M., & Wolff, W. (2021). It’s not a bug, it’s boredom: Effortful willpower balances exploitation and exploration. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X20001053 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bowman, N. D., & Tamborini, R. (2015). “In the mood to game”: Selective exposure and mood management processes in computer game play. New Media & Society, 17(3), 375–393. https://doi.org/10.1177/1461444813504274 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brissett, D., & Snow, R. P. (1993). Boredom: Where the future isn’t. Symbolic Interaction, 16(3), 237–256. https://doi.org/10.1525/si.1993.16.3.237 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bryant, J., & Zillmann, D. (1984). Using television to alleviate boredom and stress: Selective exposure as a function of induced excitational states. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 28(1), 1–20. https://doi.org/10.1080/08838158409386511 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cacioppo, J. T.Tassinary, L. G.Berntson, G. (Eds.). (2007). Handbook of psychophysiology. Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chin, A., Markey, A., Bhargava, S., Kassam, K. S., & Loewenstein, G. (2017). Bored in the USA: Experience sampling and boredom in everyday life. Emotion, 17(2), 359–368. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000232 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Clarke, J., & Draper, S. (2020). Intermittent mindfulness practice can be beneficial, and daily practice can be harmful. An in depth, mixed methods study of the “Calm” app’s (mostly positive) effects. Internet Interventions, 19, Article 100293. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.invent.2019.100293 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Coan, J. A., & Allen, J. J. (2004). Frontal EEG asymmetry as a moderator and mediator of emotion. Biological Psychology, 67(1–2), 7–50. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.03.002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Coughlan, G., Igou, E. R., van Tilburg, W. A., Kinsella, E. L., & Ritchie, T. D. (2019). On boredom and perceptions of heroes: A meaning-regulation approach to heroism. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 59(4), 455–473. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022167817705281 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Beyond boredom and anxiety. Jossey-Bass. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Danckert, J., & Eastwood, J. D. (2020). Out of my skull: The psychology of boredom. Harvard University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Danckert, J., Mugon, J., Struk, A., & Eastwood, J. (2018). Boredom: What is it good for? In H. C. LenchEd., The function of emotions (pp. 93–119). Springer. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Danckert, J., Hammerschmidt, T., Marty-Dugas, J., & Smilek, D. (2018). Boredom: Under-aroused and restless. Consciousness and Cognition, 61, 24–37. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Eastwood, J. D., Frischen, A., Fenske, M. J., & Smilek, D. (2012). The unengaged mind: Defining boredom in terms of attention. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(5), 482–495. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612456044 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fahlman, S. A., Mercer-Lynn, K. B., Flora, D. B., & Eastwood, J. D. (2013). Development and validation of the multidimensional state boredom scale. Assessment, 20(1), 68–85. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191111421303 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Farmer, R., & Sundberg, N. D. (1986). Boredom proneness – the development and correlates of a new scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 50(1), 4–17. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa5001_2 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Geisler, F. C., Vennewald, N., Kubiak, T., & Weber, H. (2010). The impact of heart rate variability on subjective well-being is mediated by emotion regulation. Personality and Individual Differences, 49(7), 723–728. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.06.015 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gläscher, J., Hampton, A. N., & O’Doherty, J. P. (2009). Determining a role for ventromedial prefrontal cortex in encoding action-based value signals during reward-related decision making. Cerebral Cortex, 19(2), 483–495. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhn098 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Review of General Psychology, 2(3), 271–299. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hill, A. B., & Perkins, R. E. (1985). Towards a model of boredom. British Journal of Psychology, 76, 235–240. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1985.tb01947.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hitchcock, E. M., Dember, W. N., Warm, J. S., Moroney, B. W., & See, J. E. (1999). Effects of cueing and knowledge of results on workload and boredom in sustained attention. Human Factors, 41, 365–372. https://doi.org/10.1518/001872099779610987 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hulleman, C. S., Godes, O., Hendricks, B. L., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2010). Enhancing interest and performance with a utility value intervention. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 880–895. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019506 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jeong, S. H., & Fishbein, M. (2007). Predictors of multitasking with media: Media factors and audience factors. Media Psychology, 10(3), 364–384. https://doi.org/10.1080/15213260701532948 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Keller, J., Bless, H., Blomann, F., & Kleinböhl, D. (2011). Physiological aspects of flow experiences: Skills-demand-compatibility effects on heart rate variability and salivary cortisol. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(4), 849–852. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2011.02.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Klimesch, W. (1999). EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: A review and analysis. Brain Research Reviews, 29(2–3), 169–195. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-0173(98)00056-3 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Knobloch, S. (2003). Mood adjustment via mass communication. Journal of Communication, 53(2), 233–250. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2003.tb02588.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lench, H. C., Flores, S. A., & Bench, S. W. (2011). Discrete emotions predict changes in cognition, judgment, experience, behavior, and physiology: A meta-analysis of experimental emotion elicitations. Psychological Bulletin, 137(5), 834–855. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024244 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lin, C. H., Lin, S. L., & Wu, C. P. (2009). The effects of parental monitoring and leisure boredom on adolescents’ internet addiction. Adolescence, 44(176), 994–1004. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • London, H., & Monello, L. (1974). Cognitive manipulation of boredom. Aldine. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Markey, A., Chin, A., Vanepps, E. M., & Loewenstein, G. (2014). Identifying a reliable boredom induction. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 119, 37–253. https://doi.org/10.2466/27.PMS.119c18z6 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mathiak, K. A., Klasen, M., Zvyagintsev, M., Weber, R., & Mathiak, K. (2013). Neural networks underlying affective states in a multimodal virtual environment: Contributions to boredom. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, Article 820. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00820 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Merrifield, C., & Danckert, J. (2014). Characterizing the psychophysiological signature of boredom. Experimental Brain Research, 232(2), 481–491. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-013-3755-2 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mikulas, W. L., & Vodanovich, S. J. (1993). The essence of boredom. The Psychological Record, 43(1), 3–12. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Mills, C., & Christoff, K. (2018). Finding consistency in boredom by appreciating its instability. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(9), 744–747. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.07.001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Milstein, N., & Gordon, I. (2020). Validating measures of electrodermal activity and heart rate variability derived from the empatica e4 utilized in research settings that involve interactive dyadic states. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 14. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2020.00148 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Moynihan, A. B., Igou, E. R., & van Tilburg, W. A. P. (2017). Boredom increases impulsiveness: A meaning-regulation perspective. Social Psychology, 48(5), 293–309. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000317 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Moynihan, A. B., Igou, E. R., & van Tilburg, W. A. (2021). Bored stiff: The relationship between meaninglessness, sexual sensation seeking, and promiscuous attitudes via boredom susceptibility. Personality and Individual Differences, 168, Article 110295. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2020.110295 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Myrick, J. G. (2015). Emotion regulation, procrastination, and watching cat videos online: Who watches Internet cats, why, and to what effect? Computers in human behavior, 52, 168–176. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.06.001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pelletier, C. L. (2004). The effect of music on decreasing arousal due to stress: A meta-analysis. Journal of music therapy, 41(3), 192–214. https://doi.org/10.1093/jmt/41.3.192 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Perone, S., Weybright, E. H., & Anderson, A. J. (2019). Over and over again: Changes in frontal EEG asymmetry across a boring task. Psychophysiology, 56(10), Article e13427. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13427 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Perone, S., Anderson, A. J., & Weybright, E. H. (2021). It is all relative: Contextual influences on boredom and neural correlates of regulatory processes. Psychophysiology, 58(3), Article e13746. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13746 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Phillips, S. W. (2016). Police discretion and boredom: What officers do when there is nothing to do. Journal of Contemporary Ethnography, 45, 580–601. https://doi.org/10.1177/0891241615587385 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Porges, S. W. (2007). The polyvagal perspective. Biological Psychology, 74(2), 116–143. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.06.009 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Potter, R. F., & Bolls, P. (2012). Psychophysiological measurement and meaning: Cognitive and emotional processing of media. Routledge. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Raffaelli, Q., Mills, C., & Christoff, K. (2018). The knowns and unknowns of boredom: A review of the literature. Experimental Brain Research, 236(9), 2451–2462. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-017-4922-7 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Reinecke, L., Hartmann, T., & Eden, A. (2014). The guilty couch potato: The role of ego depletion in reducing recovery through media use. Journal of Communication, 64(4), 569–589. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcom.12107 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Reinecke, L., Klatt, J., & Krämer, N. C. (2011). Entertaining media use and the satisfaction of recovery needs: Recovery outcomes associated with the use of interactive and noninteractive entertaining media. Media Psychology, 14(2), 192–215. https://doi.org/10.1080/15213269.2011.573466 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rieger, D., Hefner, D., & Vorderer, P. (2017). Mobile recovery? The impact of smartphone use on recovery experiences in waiting situations. Mobile Media & Communication, 5(2), 161–177. https://doi.org/10.1177/2050157917691556 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rieger, D., Reinecke, L., Frischlich, L., & Bente, G. (2014). Media entertainment and well-being – Linking hedonic and eudaimonic entertainment experience to media-induced recovery and vitality. Journal of Communication, 64(3), 456–478. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcom.12097 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rudnicki, K., Declerck, C., De Backer, C., & Berth, M. (2019). Physiological changes during first encounters and their role in determining the perceived interaction quality. Interaction Studies, 20(2), 275–306. https://doi.org/10.1075/is.18015.rud First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schrock, A. R. (2015). Communicative affordances of mobile media: Portability, availability, locatability, and multimediality. International Journal of Communication, 9, 18. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Shenhav, A., Botvinick, M. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2013). The expected value of control: An integrative theory of anterior cingulate cortex function. Neuron, 79(2), 217–240. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2013.07.007 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Shiffman, S., Stone, A. A., & Hufford, M. R. (2008). Ecological momentary assessment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 4, 1–32. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091415 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Slonje, R., Smith, P. K., & Frisén, A. (2012). Processes of cyberbullying, and feelings of remorse by bullies: A pilot study. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9(2), 244–259. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2011.643670 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Smith, E. E., Reznik, S. J., Stewart, J. L., & Allen, J. J. (2017). Assessing and conceptualizing frontal EEG asymmetry: An updated primer on recording, processing, analyzing, and interpreting frontal alpha asymmetry. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 111, 98–114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2016.11.005 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Steinberger, F., Schroeter, R., & Watling, C. N. (2017). From road distraction to safe driving: Evaluating the effects of boredom and gamification on driving behaviour, physiological arousal, and subjective experience. Computers in Human Behavior, 75, 714–726. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.06.019 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tam, K. Y., Van Tilburg, W. A., Chan, C. S., Igou, E. R., & Lau, H. (2021). Attention drifting in and out: The boredom feedback model. Personality and Social Psychology Review, , 10888683211010297. https://doi.org/10.1177/10888683211010297 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Thackray, R. I., Bailey, J. P., & Touchstone, R. M. (1977). Physiological, subjective, and performance correlates of reported boredom and monotony while performing a simulated radar control task. In R. R. MackieEd., Vigilance (pp. 203–215). Springer. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Vandebosch, H., & Poels, K. (2021). Cooling down or charging up? engagement with aggressive entertainment contents as an emotion regulation strategy of boredom and anger. In P. VordererC. KlimmtEds., Oxford handbook of entertainment theory (pp. 479–497). Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Vanden Abeele, M. M. (2021). Digital wellbeing as a dynamic construct. Communication Theory, 31(4), 932–955. https://doi.org/10.1093/ct/qtaa024 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • van der Vlist, F. N. (2016). Accounting for the social: Investigating commensuration and Big Data practices at Facebook. Big Data & Society, 3(1), 2053951716631365. https://doi.org/10.1177/2053951716631365 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • van Tilburg, W. A., & Igou, E. R. (2012). On boredom: Lack of challenge and meaning as distinct boredom experiences. Motivation and Emotion, 36(2), 181–194. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-011-9234-9 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • van Tilburg, W. A., & Igou, E. R. (2016). Going to political extremes in response to boredom. European Journal of Social Psychology, 46(6), 687–699. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2205 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • van Tilburg, W. A., & Igou, E. R. (2017). Can boredom help? Increased prosocial intentions in response to boredom. Self and Identity, 16(1), 82–96. https://doi.org/10.1080/15298868.2016.1218925 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • van Tilburg, W. A., Igou, E. R., & Sedikides, C. (2013). In search of meaningfulness: Nostalgia as an antidote to boredom. Emotion, 13(3), 450–461. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Varjas, K., Talley, J., Meyers, J., Parris, L., & Cutts, H. (2010). High school students’ perceptions of motivations for cyberbullying: An exploratory study. Western Journal of Emergency Medicine, 11(3), Article 2690273. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Vettehen, P. H., Nuijten, K., & Peeters, A. (2008). Explaining effects of sensationalism on liking of television news stories: The role of emotional arousal. Communication Research, 35(3), 319–338. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650208315960 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vorderer, P., & Reinecke, L. (2015). From mood to meaning: The changing model of the user in entertainment research. Communication Theory, 25(4), 447–453. https://doi.org/10.1111/comt.12082 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wegmann, E., Ostendorf, S., & Brand, M. (2018). Is it beneficial to use Internet-communication for escaping from boredom? Boredom proneness interacts with cue-induced craving and avoidance expectancies in explaining symptoms of Internet-communication disorder. PLoS One, 13(4), Article e0195742. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0195742 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Westgate, E. C. (2020). Why boredom is interesting. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 29(1), 33–40. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721419884309 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Westgate, E. C., & Wilson, T. D. (2018). Boring thoughts and bored minds: The MAC model of boredom and cognitive engagement. Psychological Review, 125(5), 1–25. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000097 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wolff, W., & Martarelli, C. S. (2020). Bored into depletion? Toward a tentative integration of perceived self-control exertion and boredom as guiding signals for goal-directed behavior. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15(5), 1272–1283. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620921394 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar