Lower Levels of Resistance to Change (but not Opposition to Equality) Is Related to Analytic Cognitive Style
Abstract
Abstract. In recent years, there has been increasing research attention to cognitive style differences between liberals and conservatives. While some studies have found a negative relation between conservatism and analytic thinking tendency, others have not observed such a relation. None of these studies has measured the core motives underlying conservative ideology and investigated their relation with analytic cognitive style (ACS). We predicted that ACS is related to only one of the core motives underlying conservatism (resistance to change), but not the other (opposition to equality). This hypothesis was supported in three non-Western samples (total n = 1,552). This finding may clarify why some studies found a relation between cognitive style and conservatism, while others did not.
References
1950). The authoritarian personality. New York, NY: Harper & Row.
(1981). Right-wing authoritarianism. Winnipeg, Canada: University of Manitoba Press.
(1992). Authoritarianism, religious fundamentalism, quest, and prejudice. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 2, 113–133.
(2017). The relation between different types of religiosity and analytic cognitive style. Personality and Individual Differences, 117, 267–272. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.06.013
(2017). Personality traits, income, and economic ideology. Political Psychology, 38, 1025–1041. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12349
(2017). Comment on Kahan and Corbin: Can polarization increase with actively open-minded thinking? Research & Politics, 4, 1–4. https://doi.org/10.1177/2053168016688122
(2015). Why does the cognitive reflection test (sometimes) predict utilitarian moral judgment (and other things)? Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 4, 265–284.
(2014). The “deliberative digital divide”: Opinion leadership and integrative complexity in the US political blogosphere. Political Psychology, 35, 741–755. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12201
(1999). Reflections on the revolution in France. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. (Original work published 1790).
(2009). The rising tide of conservatism in Turkey. New York, NY: Palgrave Macmillan US.
(2011). The automatic conservative: Ideology-based attentional asymmetries in the processing of valenced information. PLoS One, 6, e26456. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0026456
(2008). Individual differences in need for closure and voting behaviour. Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 1279–1288. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.11.012
(2006). The impact of cognitive styles on authoritarianism based conservatism and racism. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 28, 37–50. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15324834basp2801_4
(2008). Conflict monitoring in dual process theories of thinking. Cognition, 106, 1248–1299. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.06.002
(2015). Reflective liberals and intuitive conservatives: A look at the Cognitive Reflection Test and ideology. Judgment and Decision Making, 10, 314–331.
(2012). The political left rolls with the good; the political right confronts the bad. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Biological Sciences, 367, 640–649.
(2009).
(On the automaticity of the status quo . In J. T. JostA. C. KayH. ThórisdóttirEds., Social and psychological bases of ideology and system justification (pp. 85–105). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.2012). Low-effort thought promotes political conservatism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 808–820. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167212439213
(2009). The existence bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 765–775. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017058
(2014). Opposition to equality and support for tradition as mediators of the relationship between epistemic motivation and system-justifying identifications. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 17, 524–541. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430213517273
(2014). Understanding the determinants of political ideology: Implications of structural complexity. Political Psychology, 35, 337–358.
(2005). Cognitive reflection and decision making. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 19, 25–42.
(2016). Has the standard cognitive reflection test become a victim of its own success? Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 12, 145–149. https://doi.org/10.5709/acp-0193-5
(2010). The weirdest people in the world? The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 61–83.
(2014). Differences in negativity bias underlie variations in political ideology. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 37, 297–307. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X13001192
(2012). Understanding libertarian morality: The psychological dispositions of self-identified libertarians. PLoS One, 7, e42366. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0042366
(2006). The end of the end of ideology. The American Psychologist, 61, 651–670.
(2003). Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 339–375. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.129.3.339
(2007). Are needs to manage uncertainty and threat associated with political conservatism or ideological extremity? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 989–1007. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167207301028
(2018).
(Getting closure on conservatism, or the politics of epistemic and existential motivation . In C. E. KopetzA. FishbachEds., The motivation-cognition interface; From the lab to the real world: A Festschrift in honor of Arie W. Kruglanski (pp. 56–87). New York, NY: Routledge/Taylor & Francis.2013). Ideology, motivated reasoning, and cognitive reflection. Judgment and Decision Making, 8, 407–424.
(1997). Need for closure and political orientation among German university students. Journal of Social Psychology, 137, 787–789. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224549709595501
(1983). Affirmative action attitudes: Effects of self-interest, racial affect, and stratification beliefs on whites’ views. Social Forces, 61, 797–824.
(2004). The psychology of closed mindedness. New York, NY: Psychology Press.
(1983). The freezing and unfreezing of lay inferences: Effects of impressional primacy, ethnic stereotyping, and numerical anchoring. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 19, 448–468. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(83)90022-7
(2007). Gençlerin siyasal ve kültürel tutumları-Ankara örneği
([Political and cultural attitudes of Turkish youth: The case of Ankara] (Unpublished master thesis). Gazi University, Ankara, Turkey.2004). Deliver us from evil: The effects of mortality salience and reminders of 9/11 on support for President George W. Bush. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1136–1150.
(2016). Representations of moral violations: Category members and associated features. Judgment and Decision Making, 11, 496–508.
(2013, September). Calculation for the test of the difference between two dependent correlations with one variable in common. [Computer software]. Retrieved from http://quantpsy.org
(2014). Do needs for security and certainty predict cultural and economic conservatism? A cross-national analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106, 1031–1051. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036170
(2017). Group-focused morality is associated with limited conflict detection and resolution capacity: Neuroanatomical evidence. Biological Psychology, 123, 235–240. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.12.018
(2015). The Moral Foundations taxonomy: Structural validity and relation to political ideology in Sweden. Personality and Individual Differences, 76, 28–32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.11.049
(2006). Changes and continuities in the Turkish party system. Representation, 42, 129–137. https://doi.org/10.1080/00344890600736366
(2010). Running experiments on Amazon Mechanical Turk. Judgment and Decision Making, 5, 411–419.
(2014). The role of analytic thinking in moral judgements and values. Thinking & Reasoning, 20, 188–214. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2013.865000
(2012). Analytic cognitive style predicts religious and paranormal belief. Cognition, 123, 335–346. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.03.003
(2016). Commentary: Cognitive reflection vs. calculation in decision making. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00009
(2014). Religiosity, political orientation, and consequentialist moral thinking. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 5, 334–342. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550613492826
(1994). Social dominance orientation: A personality variable predicting social and political attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 741–763.
(2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 717–731. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03206553
(2017). Kişisel ve toplumsal düzeylerde eşitliğe karşıtlık ve değişime direnmenin muhafazakârlıkla ilişkisi
([The relationship between conservatism and opposition to equality and resistance to change at the personal and societal levels] . Turkish Psychological Articles,[Türk Psikoloji Yazıları] , 39, 24–41.2017). Analytic cognitive style and cognitive ability differentially predict religiosity and social conservatism. Personality and Individual Differences, 114, 24–29. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.03.056
(1986). College sophomores in the laboratory: Influences of a narrow data base on social psychology’s view of human nature. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 515–530. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.51.3.515
(2015). Cognitive reflection vs. calculation in decision making. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 532. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00532
(2017). Replacing the moral foundations: An evolutionary‐coalitional theory of liberal‐conservative differences. Political Psychology, 38, 1043–1064.
(2016). A limitation of the Cognitive Reflection Test: familiarity. PeerJ, 4, e2395. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.2395
(2016). Are neoliberals more susceptible to bullshit? Judgment and Decision Making, 11, 352–360.
(1994). Conservatism/liberalism. Encyclopedia of Human Behavior, 1, 701–707.
(1977). Integrative complexity of communications in international crises. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 21, 169–184. https://doi.org/10.1177/002200277702100108
(2015). Liberals think more analytically (more “WEIRD”) than conservatives. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41, 250–267. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167214563672
(1983). Accountability and complexity of thought. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 74–83. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.45.1.74
(2016). Investigating an alternate form of the cognitive reflection test. Judgment and Decision Making, 11, 99–113.
(2011). Motivated closed-mindedness mediates the effect of threat on political conservatism. Political Psychology, 32, 785–811. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2011.00840.x
(2015). Hierarchy, dominance, and deliberation: Egalitarian values require mental effort. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41, 1207–1222. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167215591961
(1994). Individual differences in need for cognitive closure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 1049–1062. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.67.6.1049
(1968). A new measure of conservatism. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 8, 264–269. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8260.1968.tb00568.x
(2016). An attempt to clarify the link between cognitive style and political ideology: A non-western replication and extension. Judgment and Decision Making, 11, 287–300.
(2016). Political orientations, ideological self-categorizations, party preferences, and moral foundations of young Turkish voters. Turkish Studies, 17, 544–566. https://doi.org/10.1080/14683849.2016.1221312
(2017a). Analytic thought training promotes liberalism on contextualized (but not stable) political opinions. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 8, 789–795. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550616687092
(2017b). The relationship between cognitive style and political orientation depends on the measures used. Judgment and Decision Making, 12, 140–147.
(2017c). Activating analytic thinking enhances the value given to individualizing moral foundations. Cognition, 165, 88–96. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2017.05.009
(2011). The fluency of social hierarchy: The ease with which hierarchical relations are seen, remembered, learned, and liked. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 98–115.
(