Skip to main content
Tutorial

Formalizing Verbal Theories

A Tutorial by Dialogue

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000428

Abstract. We present a tutorial for formalizing verbal theories of psychological phenomena – social or otherwise. The approach builds on concepts and tools from the mathematics of computation. We use intuitive examples and illustrate the intrinsic dialectical nature of the formalization process by presenting dialogues between two fictive characters, called Verbal and Formal. These characters’ conversations and thought experiments serve to highlight important lessons in theoretical modeling.

References

  • Baker, C. L., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Saxe, R. R. (2007). Goal inference as inverse planning. 29th Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 779–784. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Blokpoel, M. (2018). Sculpting computational-level models. Topics in Cognitive Science, 10(3), 641–648. https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12282 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Blokpoel, M., Dingemanse, M., Woensdregt, M., Kachergis, G., Bögels, S., Toni, I., & van Rooij, I. (2019). Pragmatic communicators can overcome asymmetry by exploiting ambiguity. Open Science Framework. https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/q56xs First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Blokpoel, M., Kwisthout, J., van der Weide, T. P., Wareham, T., & van Rooij, I. (2013). A computational-level explanation of the speed of goal inference. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 57(3), 117–133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jmp.2013.05.006 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cooper, R. P., & Guest, O. (2014). Implementations are not specifications: Specification, replication and experimentation in computational cognitive modeling. Cognitive Systems Research, 27, 42–49. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogsys.2013.05.001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cummins, R. (1985). The nature of psychological explanation, MIT Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Cummins, R. (2000). How does it work?” versus” what are the laws?”: Two conceptions of psychological explanation. In F. C. KeilR. A. WilsonEds., Explanation and cognition (pp. 117–144). MIT Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • De Houwer, J., & Moors, A. (2015). Levels of analysis in social psychology. In B. GawronskiG. V. BodenhausenEds., Theory and explanation in social psychology (pp. 24–40). Guilford Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Guest, O., & Martin, A. E. (2020). How computational modeling can force theory building in psychological science. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/rybh9 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Klapper, A., Dotsch, R., van Rooij, I., & Wigboldus, D. H. (2018). Social categorization in connectionist models: A conceptual integration. Social Cognition, 36(2), 221–246. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2018.36.2.221 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Knuth, D. E. (1968). The art of computer programming: Sorting and Searching. Addison-Wesley. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Macal, C. M., & North, M. J. (2010). Tutorial on agent-based modelling and simulation. Journal of Simulation, 4(3), 151–162. https://doi.org/10.1057/jos.2010.3 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Marr, D. (1982). Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information. W. H. Freeman. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Mitchell, J. P. (2006). Mentalizing and Marr: An information processing approach to the study of social cognition. Brain Research, 1079(1), 66–75. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2005.12.113 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Navarro, D. J. (2019). Between the devil and the deep blue sea: Tensions between scientific judgment and statistical model selection. Computational Brain & Behavior, 2(1), 28–34. https://doi.org/10.1007/s42113-018-0019-z First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Otworowska, M., Blokpoel, M., Sweers, M., Wareham, T., & van Rooij, I. (2018). Demons of ecological rationality. Cognitive Science, 42(3), 1057–1066. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12530 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Perfors, A., & Navarro, D. J. (2019). Why do echo chambers form? The role of trust, population heterogeneity, and objective truth. In A. GoelC. SeifertC. FreksaEds., Proceedings of the 41st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 918–923). Cognitive Science Society. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Read, S. J., & Miller, L. C. (1998). Connectionist models of social reasoning and social behavior, Psychology Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Rich, P., Blokpoel, M., de Haan, R., Otworowska, M., Sweers, M., Wareham, T., & van Rooij, I. (2019). Naturalism, tractability and the adaptive toolbox. Synthese. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-019-02431-2 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rich, P., Blokpoel, M., de Haan, R., & van Rooij, I. (2020). How intractability spans the cognitive and evolutionary levels of explanation. Topics in Cognitive Science. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12506 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ross, L. (1977). The intuitive psychologist and his [sic] shortcomings: Distortions in the attribution process. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 10, 173–220. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60357-3 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Smith, E. R., & Conrey, F. R. (2007). Agent-based modeling: A new approach for theory-building in social psychology. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11, 87–104. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Szollosi, A., Kellen, D., Navarro, D. J., Shiffrin, R., van Rooij, I., Van Zandt, T., & Donkin, C. (2020). Is peregistration worthwhile? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 24(2), 94–95. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2019.11.009 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Thagard, P. (2000). Coherence in thought and action, The MIT Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Thagard, P. (2006). Hot thought, The MIT Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Thagard, P., & Kunda, Z. (1998). Making sense of people: Coherence mechanisms. In S. J. ReadL. C. MillerEds., Connectionist models of social reasoning and social behavior (pp. 3–26). Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • van Rooij, I. (2008). The tractable cognition thesis. Cognitive Science, 32(6), 939–984. https://doi.org/10.1080/03640210801897856 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • van Rooij, I., & Baggio, G. (in press). Theory before the test: How to build high-verisimilitude explanatory theories in psychological science. Perspectives on Psychological Science. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/7qbpr First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • van Rooij, I., Blokpoel, M., Kwisthout, J., & Wareham, T. (2019). Cognition and intractability: A guide to classical and parameterized complexity analysis, Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781107358331 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Woensdregt, M., Cummins, C., & Smith, K. A. (2020). A computational model of the cultural co-evolution of language and mindreading. Synthese. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-020-02798-7 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar