Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000430

Abstract. This article explores people’s perception of their experience while being alone or being with others. To that aim, data from nine studies (total N = 1,717) were meta-analyzed. In each study, participants performed a sentence-completion task referring to their experience alone/“with others”. The responses were analyzed using a text analysis software. Results showed that in a “with others” (vs. alone) mindset, people are more focused on the present, and less focused on the past or the future. When “with others,” more anxiety but less sadness is expressed. Social motives – affiliation and power – are more pronounced in the “with others” mindset, whereas leisure activities are salient in the alone mindset. Conclusions are derived regarding the nature of the experience in these basic conditions.

References

  • Argyle, M., & Lu, L. (1990). The happiness of extraverts. Personality and Individual Differences, 11(10), 1011–1017. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(90)90128-E First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baron, R. S. (1986). Distraction-conflict theory: Progress and problems. In L. BerkowitzEd., Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 19, pp. 1–40). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60211-7 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Baumeister, R. F., Hofmann, W., Summerville, A., Reiss, P. T., & Vohs, K. D. (2020). Everyday thoughts in time: Experience sampling studies of mental time travel. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167220908411 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Berenbaum, H. (2002). Varieties of joy-related pleasurable activities and feelings. Cognition & Emotion, 16(4), 473–494. https://doi.org/10.1080/0269993014000383 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Blascovich, J., Mendes, W. B., Hunter, S. B., & Salomon, K. (1999). Social “facilitation” as challenge and threat. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(1), 68–77. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.77.1.68 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bodenhausen, G. V., Sheppard, L. A., & Kramer, G. P. (1994). Negative affect and social judgment: The differential impact of anger and sadness. European Journal of Social Psychology, 24(1), 45–62. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2420240104 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bond, C. F., & Titus, L. J. (1983). Social facilitation: A meta-analysis of 241 studies. Psychological Bulletin, 94(2), 265–292. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.94.2.265 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Burger, J. M. (1995). Individual differences in preference for solitude. Journal of Research in Personality, 29, 85–108. https://doi.org/10.1006/jrpe.1995.1005 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Coplan, R. J.Bowker, J. C. (Eds.). (2014). The handbook of solitude: Psychological perspectives on social isolation, social withdrawal, and being alone, Wiley. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Cottrell, N. B. (1968). Performance in the presence of other human beings: Mere presence and affiliation effects. In E. C. SimmelR. A. HoppeG. A. MiltonEds., Social facilitation and imitative behavior (pp. 91–110). Allyn and Bacon. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Erber, R., Wegner, D. M., & Therriault, N. (1996). On being cool and collected: Mood regulation in anticipation of social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(4), 757–766. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.70.4.757 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 1149–1160. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fiske, S. T. (2018). Stereotype content: Warmth and competence endure. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27(2), 67–73. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721417738825 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gable, S. L., & Reis, H. T. (2001). Appetitive and aversive social interaction. In J. H. HarveyA. E. WenzelEds., Close romantic relationship maintenance and enhancement (pp. 169–194). Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Gable, S. L., Reis, H. T., & Downey, G. (2003). He said, she said: A quasi-signal detection analysis of daily interactions between close relationship partners. Psychological Science, 14(2), 100–105. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.t01-1-01426 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Guerin, B. (1986). Mere presence effects in humans: A review. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 22(1), 38–77. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(86)90040-5 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Guerin, B. (1993). Social facilitation, Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Guerin, B., & Innes, J. M. (1982). Social facilitation and social monitoring: A new look at Zajonc’s mere presence hypothesis. British Journal of Social Psychology, 21(1), 7–18. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.1982.tb00506.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Henchy, T., & Glass, D. C. (1968). Evaluation apprehension and the social facilitation of dominant responses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 10, 446–454. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0026814 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Heser, K., Banse, R., & Imhoff, R. (2015). Affiliation or Power: What motivates behavior on social networking sites? Swiss Journal of Psychology, 74(1), 37–47. https://doi.org/10.1024/1421-0185/a000144 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Inzlicht, M., & Schmeichel, B. J. (2012). What is ego depletion? Toward a mechanistic revision of the resource model of self-control. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(5), 450–463. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612454134 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jun, Y., Meng, R., & Johar, G. V. (2017). Perceived social presence reduces fact-checking. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(23), 5976–5981. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1700175114 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Korpela, K., & Staats, H. (2014). The restorative qualities of being alone with nature. In R. J. CoplanJ. C. BowkerEds., The handbook of solitude (pp. 351–367). Wiley-Blackwell. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kramer, S., Lewin, K. M., Romano, A. S., & Meier, B. P. (2020). I saw that: Being observed reduces race-based shoot decisions. Social Psychology, 51, 141–148. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000402 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Larson, R. W. (1990). The solitary side of life: An examination of the time people spend alone from childhood to old age. Developmental Review, 10, 155–183. https://doi.org/10.1016/0273-2297(90)90008-R First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Larson, R. W. (1997). The emergence of solitude as a constructive domain of experience in early adolescence. Child Development, 68, 80–93. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131927 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lay, J. C., Pauly, T., Graf, P., Biesanz, J. C., & Hoppmann, C. A. (2019). By myself and liking it? Predictors of distinct types of solitude experiences in daily life. Journal of Personality, 87(3), 633–647. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12421 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lieberman, M. D., & Rosenthal, R. (2001). Why introverts can’t always tell who likes them: Multitasking and nonverbal decoding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(2), 294–310. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.80.2.294 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Long, C. R., & Averill, J. R. (2003). Solitude: An exploration of benefits of being alone. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 33(1), 21–44. https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-5914.00204 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Long, C. R., Seburn, M., Averill, J. R., & More, T. A. (2003). Solitude experiences: Varieties, settings, and individual differences. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(5), 578–583. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167203029005003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Matias, G. P., Nicolson, N. A., & Freire, T. (2011). Solitude and cortisol: Associations with state and trait affect in daily life. Biological Psychology, 86(3), 314–319. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.12.011 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McCrea, S. M., Liberman, N., Trope, Y., & Sherman, S. J. (2008). Construal level and procrastination. Psychological Science, 19(12), 1308–1314. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02240.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nguyen, T. V. T., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2018). Solitude as an approach to affective self-regulation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 44, 92–106. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167217733073 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Noah, T., Schul, Y., & Mayo, R. (2018). Thinking of oneself as an object of observation reduces reliance on metacognitive information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147(7), 1023–1042. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000440 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nyer, P. U., & Dellande, S. (2010). Public commitment as a motivator for weight loss. Psychology & Marketing, 27(1), 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1002/mar.20316 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Paulus, P. B., & Yang, H. C. (2000). Idea generation in groups: A basis for creativity in organizations. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 82(1), 76–87. https://doi.org/10.1006/obhd.2000.2888 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pauly, T., Lay, J. C., Nater, U. M., Scott, S. B., & Hoppmann, C. A. (2017). How we experience being alone: Age differences in affective and biological correlates of momentary solitude. Gerontology, 63(1), 55–66. https://doi.org/10.1159/000450608 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pennebaker, J. W., Booth, R. J., & Francis, M. E. (2007). Linguistic inquiry and word count (LIWC): LIWC2007, LIWC.net. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Pennebaker, J. W., Boyd, R. L., Jordan, K., & Blackburn, K. (2015). The development and psychometric properties of LIWC2015, University of Texas at Austin. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Pennebaker, J. W., Francis, M. E., & Booth, R. J. (2001). Linguistic inquiry and word count: LIWC, Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Pennebaker, J. W., Mehl, M. R., & Niederhoffer, K. G. (2003). Psychological aspects of natural language use: Our words, our selves. Annual Review of Psychology, 54(1), 547–577. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145041 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Plaks, J. E., Levy, S. R., & Dweck, C. S. (2009). Lay theories of personality: Cornerstones of meaning in social cognition. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 3(6), 1069–1081. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2009.00222.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Quirin, M., Meyer, F., Heise, N., Kuhl, J., Küstermann, E., Strüber, D., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2013). Neural correlates of social motivation: An fMRI study on power versus affiliation. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 88(3), 289–295. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.07.003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • R Core Team. (2018). R: A Language and environment for statistical computing [Computer software]. https://cran.r-project.org/ First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Rook, K. S. (1984). Promoting social bonding: Strategies for helping the lonely and socially isolated. American Psychologist, 39(12), 1389–1407. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.39.12.1389 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sauter, D. (2010). More than happy: The need for disentangling positive emotions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19(1), 36–40. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721409359290 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Steinmetz, J., Xu, Q., Fishbach, A., & Zhang, Y. (2016). Being observed magnifies action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 111(6), 852–865. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000065 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • The jamovi project. (2019). jamovi. (Version 1.0) [Computer software]. https://www.jamovi.org First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Triplett, N. (1898). The dynamogenic factors in pacemaking and competition. American Journal of Psychology, 9, 507–533. https://doi.org/10.2307/1412188 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2010). Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychological Review, 117(2), 440–463. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018963 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Uziel, L. (2007). Individual differences in the social facilitation effect: A review and meta-analysis. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 579–601. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2006.06.008 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Uziel, L. (2016). Alone, unhappy, and demotivated: The impact of an alone mind-set on neurotic individuals’ willpower. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 7, 818–827. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550616657597 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Uziel, L., & Baumeister, R. F. (2012). The effect of public social context on self-control: Depletion for neuroticism and restoration for impression management. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(3), 384–396. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167211427310 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Uziel, L., & Hefetz, U. (2014). The selfish side of self-control. European Journal of Personality, 28(5), 449–458. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.1972 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Uziel, L., Seemann, M., & Schmidt-Barad, T. (2020). From being alone to being the only one: Neuroticism is associated with an egocentric shift in an alone context. Journal of Personality, 88, 339–355. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12494 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Valentine, J. C., Pigott, T. D., & Rothstein, H. R. (2010). How many studies do you need? A primer on statistical power for meta-analysis. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 35(2), 215–247. https://doi.org/10.3102/1076998609346961 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wakslak, C., & Trope, Y. (2009). The effect of construal level on subjective probability estimates. Psychological Science, 20(1), 52–58. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02250.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • West, M. A. (2002). Sparkling fountains or stagnant ponds: An integrative model of creativity and innovation implementation in work groups. Applied Psychology, 51(3), 355–387. https://doi.org/10.1111/1464-0597.00951 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wilson, T. D., Reinhard, D. A., Westgate, E. C., Gilbert, D. T., Ellerbeck, N., Hahn, C., Brown, C. L., & Shaked, A. (2014). Just think: The challenges of the disengaged mind. Science, 345, 75–77. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1250830 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Winnicott, D. (1958). The capacity to be alone. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 39, 416–420. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Winter, D. G., John, O. P., Stewart, A. J., Klohnen, E. C., & Duncan, L. E. (1998). Traits and motives: Toward an integration of two traditions in personality research. Psychological Review, 105(2), 230–250. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.105.2.230 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zajonc, R. B. (1965). Social facilitation. Science, 149, 269–274. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.149.3681.269 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar