Skip to main content
Original Article

A Threat in the Classroom

Gender Stereotype Activation and Mental-Rotation Performance in Elementary-School Children

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/a000097

Females’ performance in a gender-stereotyped domain is impaired when negative gender stereotypes are activated (Nguyen & Ryan, 2008). “Stereotype threat” affects the gender difference in adults’ mental-rotation performance (e.g., Moè & Pazzaglia, 2006). Our study investigated this effect in fourth graders. Two hundred sixteen males and females solved two mental-rotation tests. In between, a gender-difference instruction was given (“boys better,” “girls better,” “no gender difference”). A significant interaction of time and gender was found in the “girls better”-condition and in the “no gender difference”-condition: As expected, the male performance advantage disappeared after these two instructions, because girls improved and boys deteriorated. Thus, the study suggests that the gender effect in mental rotation is affected by stereotype threat and stereotype lift from the very beginning of its occurrence. Results are discussed within a biopsychosocial framework and seem to play an important role with regard to the “hidden curriculum” in schools.

References

  • Alter, A. L. , Aronson, J. , Darley, J. M. , Rodriguez, C. , Ruble, D. N. (2010). Rising to the threat: Reducing stereotype threat by reframing the threat as a challenge. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 166–171. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ambady, N. , Shih, M. , Kim, A. , Pittinsky, T. L. (2001). Stereotype susceptibility in children: Effects of identity activation on quantitative performance. Psychological Science, 12, 385–390. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Archibald, A. B. , Graber, J. A. , Brooks-Gunn, J. (2006). Pubertal processes and physiological growth in adolescence. In G. R. Adams, M. D. Berzonsky, (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of adolescence (2nd ed., pp. 24–47). Malden, MA: Blackwell. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Basow, S. (2004). The hidden curriculum: Gender in the classroom. In M. A. Paludi, (Ed.), Praeger guide to the psychology of gender (pp. 117–131). Westport, CT: Greenwood. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Basow, S. (2010). Gender in the classroom. In J. C. Chrisler, D. R. McCreary, (Eds.), Handbook of gender research in psychology: Gender research in general and experimental psychology, (Vol. 1, pp. 277–295). New York, NY: Springer. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bethell-Fox, C. E. , Shepard, R. N. (1988). Mental rotation: Effects of stimulus complexity and familiarity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1173–1182. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Blanton, H. , Christie, C. , Dye, M. (2002). Social identity versus reference frame comparisons: The moderating role of stereotype endorsement. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 253–267. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bock, R. D. , Kolakowski, D. (1973). Further evidence of sex-linked major-gene influence on human spatial visualizing ability. American Journal of Human Genetics, 25, 1–14. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bussey, K. , Bandura, A. (1999). Social cognitive theory of gender development and differentiation. Psychological Review, 106, 676–713. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chalabaev, A. , Stone, J. , Sarrazin, P. (2008). Investigating physiological and self-reported measures of stereotype lift effects on a motor task. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 30, 18–26. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chatard, A. , Selimbegovic, L. , Konan, P. , Mugny, G. (2008). Performance boosts in the classroom: Stereotype endorsement and prejudice moderate stereotype lift. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 44, 1421–1424. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Cohen, J. (1977). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Cvencek, D. , Meltzoff, A. N. , Greenwald, A. G. (2011). Math-gender stereotypes in elementary school children. Child Development, 82, 766–779. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Damon, W. , Hart, D. (1988). Self-understanding in childhood and adolescence. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Désert, M. , Préaux, M. , Jund, R. (2009). So young and already victims of stereotype threat: Socio-economic status and performance of 6 to 9 years old children on Raven’s progressive matrices. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 24, 207–218. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Else-Quest, N. M. , Hyde, J. S. , & Linn, M. C. (2010). Cross-national patterns of gender differences in mathematics: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 103–127. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Estes, Z. , Felker, S. (2011). Confidence mediates the sex difference in mental rotation performance. Archives of Sexual Behavior. doi: 10.1007/s10508-011-9875-5. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Geiser, C. , Lehmann, W. , & Eid, M. (2008). A note on sex differences in mental rotation in different age groups. Intelligence, 36, 556–563. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gooden, A. M. , Gooden, M. A. (2001). Gender representation in notable children’s pictures books: 1995–1999. Sex Roles, 45, 89–101. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Grimshaw, G. M. , Sitarenios, G. , Finegan, J.-A. K. (1995). Mental rotation at 7 years: Relations with prenatal testosterone levels and spatial play experiences. Brain and Cognition, 29, 85–100. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Halari, R. , Hines, M. , Kumari, V. , Mehrotra, R. , Wheeler, M. , Ng, V. , Sharma, T. (2005). Sex differences and individual differences in cognitive performance and their relationship to endogenous gonadal hormones and gonadotropins. Behavioral Neuroscience, 119, 104–117. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Halpern, D. F. (2000). Sex differences in cognitive abilities (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Halpern, D. F. , Wai, J. , Saw, A. (2005). A psychobiosocial model: Why females are sometimes greater than and sometimes less than males in math achievement. In A. M. Gallagher, J. C. Kaufman, (Eds.), Gender differences in mathematics: An integrative psychological approach (pp. 48–72). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hausmann, M. , Schoofs, D. , Rosenthal, H. E. S. , Jordan, K. (2009). Interactive effects of sex hormones and gender stereotypes on cognitive sex differences – a psychobiosocial approach. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34, 389–401. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hausmann, M. , Slabbekorn, D. , Van Goosen, S. H. M. , Cohen-Kettenis, P. T. , Güntürkün, O. (2000). Sex hormones affect spatial abilities during the menstrual cycle. Behavioral Neuroscience, 114, 1245–1250. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Heller, K. , Geisler, H.-J. (1983). KFT 1-3. Kognitiver Fähigkeits-Test Grundschulform [Cognitive ability test, elementary school version]. Weinheim, Germany: Beltz Test GmbH. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hines, M. , Fane, B. A. , Pasterski, V. L. , Mathews, G. A. , Conway, G. S. , Brooke, C. (2003). Spatial abilities following prenatal androgen abnormality: Targeting and mental rotations performance in individuals with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 28, 1010–1026. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ilan, A. B. , Miller, J. (1994). A violation of pure insertion: Mental rotation and choice reaction time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Psychophysics, 20, 520–536. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jöckel, K.-H. , Babitsch, B. , Bellach, B.-M. , Bloomfield, K. , Hoffmeyer-Zlotnik, J. , Winkler, J. , Wolf, C. (1998). Messung und Quantifizierung soziodemografischer Merkmale in epidemiologischen Studien [Measurement and quantification of sociodemographic attributes in epidemiological studies]. In W. Ahrens, B.-M. Bellach, K.-H. Jöckel, (Eds.), Messung soziodemografischer Merkmale in der Epidemiologie (pp. 7–38). München, Germany: MMV Münchner Medizin Verlag. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Johnson, W. , Zava, D. , McCoy, N. (2000). Overall self-confidence, self-confidence in mathematics, and sex-role stereotyping in relation to salivary free testosterone in university women. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 91, 391–401. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jovanovic, J. , King, S. S. (1998). Boys and girls in the performance-based science classroom: Who’s doing the performance? American Educational Research Journal, 35, 477–496. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Levine, S. C. , Huttenlocher, J. , Taylor, A. , Langrock, A. (1999). Early sex differences in spatial skill. Developmental Psychology, 35, 940–949. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Linn, M. C. , Petersen, A. C. (1985). Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 56, 1479–1498. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Maccoby, E. E. , Jacklin, C. N. (1974). The psychology of sex differences. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Maki, P. M. , Rich, J. B. , Rosenbaum, R. S. (2002). Implicit memory varies across the menstrual cycle: Estrogen effects in young women. Neuropsychologia, 40, 518–529. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Marsh, H. W. , Barnes, J. , Cairns, L. , & Tidman, M. (1984). The Self Description Questionnaire (SDQ): Age effects in the structure and level of self-concept for preadolescent children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 940–956. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Martinot, D. , Désert, M. (2007). Awareness of a gender stereotype, personal beliefs and self-perceptions regarding math ability: When boys do not surpass girls. Social Psychology of Education, 10, 455–471. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Masters, M. S. , Sanders, B. (1993). Is the gender difference in mental rotation disappearing? Behavior Genetics, 23, 337–341. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McGee, M. (1979). Human spatial abilities: Psychometric studies and environmental, genetic, hormonal, and neurological influences. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 889–918. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McGlone, M. S. , Aronson, J. (2006). Stereotype threat, identity salience, and spatial reasoning. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 27, 486–493. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McKown, C. , Weinstein, R. (2003). The development and consequences of stereotype consciousness in middle childhood. Child Development, 74, 498–515. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Moè, A. (2009). Are males always better than females in mental rotation? Exploring a gender belief explanation. Learning and Individual Differences, 19, 21–27. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Moè, A. (2012). Gender difference does not mean genetic difference: Externalizing improves performance in mental rotation. Learning and Individual Differences, 22, 20–24. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Moè, A. , Pazzaglia, F. (2006). Following the instructions! Effects of gender beliefs in mental rotation. Learning and Individual Differences, 16, 369–377. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Moè, A. , Pazzaglia, F. (2010). Beyond genetics in mental rotation test performance. The power of effort attribution. Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 464–468. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Muzzatti, B. , Agnoli, F. (2007). Gender and mathematics: Attitudes and stereotype threat susceptibility in Italian children. Developmental Psychology, 43, 747–759. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nash, S. C. (1979). Sex role as mediator of intellectual functioning. In M. A. Wittig, A. C. Peterson, (Eds.), Sex-related differences in cognitive functioning (pp. 263–302). New York, NY: Academic Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Neuburger, S. , Jansen, P. , Heil, M. , Quaiser-Pohl, C. (2011). Gender differences in pre-adolescents’ mental-rotation performance: Do they depend on grade and stimulus type? Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 1238–1242. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Neuburger, S. , Jansen, P. , Heil, M. , Quaiser-Pohl, C. (2012). Acceptance and effects of role models in the spatial domain: A storytelling experiment with fourth graders. Manuscript submitted for publication. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Nguyen, H. D. , Ryan, A. M. (2008). Does stereotype threat affect test performance of minorities and women? A meta-analysis of experimental evidence. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93, 1314–1334. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ortner, T. M. , Sieverding, M. (2008). Where are the gender differences? Male priming boosts spatial skills in women. Sex Roles, 59, 274–281. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Peters, M. , Laeng, B. , Latham, K. , Jackson, M. , Zaiyouna, R. , Richardson, C. (1995). A redrawn Vandenberg and Kuse mental rotations test: Different versions and factors that affect performance. Brain and Cognition, 28, 39–58. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pezaris, E. , Casey, M. B. (1991). Girls who use “masculine” problem-solving strategies on a spatial task: Proposed genetic and environmental factors. Brain and Cognition, 17, 1–22. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Quinn, P. C. , Liben, L. S. (2008). A sex difference in mental rotation in young infants. Psychological Science, 19, 1067–1070. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rahman, Q. , Wilson, G. D. , Abrahams, S. (2004). Biosocial factors, sexual orientation and neurocognitive functioning. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 29, 867–881. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Reiter, E. O. , Grumbach, M. M. (1982). Neuroendocrine control mechanisms and the onset of puberty. Annual Review of Physiology, 44, 595–613. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rosenthal, R. , Fode, K. L. (1963). Three experiments in experimenter bias. Psychological Reports, 12, 491–511. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Rosenthal, R. , Jacobson, L. (1968). Pygmalion in the classroom: Teacher expectation and pupils’ intellectual development. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ruble, D. N. (1987). The acquisition of self-knowledge: A self-socialization perspective. In N. Eisenberg, (Ed.), Contemporary topics in developmental psychology. New York, NY: Wiley. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ruble, D. N. , Martin, C. L. (1998). Gender development. In N. Eisenberg, (Ed.) & W. Damon, (Series Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Social, emotional, and personality development (Vol. 3, 5th ed., pp. 993–1016). New York, NY: Wiley. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Sadker, D. M. , Zittleman, K. (2007). Practical strategies for detecting and correcting gender bias in your classroom. In D. M. Sadker, E. S. Silber, (Eds.), Gender in the classroom: Foundations, skills, methods, and strategies across the curriculum (pp. 259–275). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Saucier, D. M. , McCreary, D. R. , Saxberg, J. K. J. (2002). Does gender role socialization mediate sex differences in mental rotations? Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 1101–1111. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schmader, T. , Johns, M. , Forbes, C. (2008). An integrated process model of stereotype threat effects on performance. Psychological Review, 115, 336–356. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sharps, M. J. , Welton, A. L. , Price, J. L. (1993). Gender and task in the determination of spatial cognitive performance. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 17, 71–83. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Shepard, R. N. , Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science, 171, 701–703. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Shih, M. , Ambady, N. , Richeson, J. A. , Fujita, K. , Gray, H. M. (2002). Stereotype performance boosts: The impact of self-relevance and the manner of stereotype activation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 638–647. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Shih, M. , Pittinsky, T. L. , Ambady, N. (1999). Stereotype susceptibility: Identity salience and shifts in quantitative performance. Psychological Science, 10, 80–83. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sims, V. K. , Mayer, R. E. (2002). Domain specificity of spatial expertise: The case of video game players. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 16, 97–115. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Spencer, S. J. , Steele, C. M. , Quinn, D. M. (1999). Stereotype threat and women’s math performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 4–28. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air. American Psychologist, 52, 613–629. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Steele, C. M. , Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African-Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 797–811. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Steele, J. (2003). Children’s gender stereotypes about math: The role of stereotype stratification. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 33, 2587–2606. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stone, J. (2002). Battling doubt by avoiding practice: The effect of stereotype threat on self-handicapping in white athletes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1667–1678. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Titze, C. , Jansen, P. , Heil, M. (2010). Mental rotation performance and the effect of gender in fourth graders and adults. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7, 432–444. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tomasetto, C. , Alparone, F. R. , Cadinu, M. (2011). Girls’ math performance under stereotype threat: The moderating role of mothers gender stereotypes. Developmental Psychology, 47, 943–949. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vandenberg, S. G. , Kuse, A. R. (1978). Mental rotations. A group test of three-dimensional spatial visualization. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 47, 599–604. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Voyer, D. , Voyer, S. , Bryden, M. P. (1995). Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: A meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 250–270. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Walton, G. M. , Cohen, G. L. (2003). Stereotype lift. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 456–467. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wigfield, A. , Eccles, J. S. , Yoon, K. S. , Harold, R. D. , Abreton, A. J. A. , Freedman-Doan, C. , Blumenfeld, P. C. (1997). Change in children’s competence beliefs and subjective task values across the elementary school years: A three-year study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 451–469. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar