Skip to main content
Review Article

A Systematic Review of Theory of Mind’s Precursors and Functions

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/a000325

Abstract. We conducted a systematic review of longitudinal theory of mind (ToM) studies, focusing on the precursors to and functional outcomes of ToM in typically-developing samples. Our search yielded 87 longitudinal studies, all of which involved children and adolescents. Early attention skills, executive function development, and the use of language are precursors to ToM development. Additionally, quality interaction with parents and older siblings can foster early improvements in ToM. Healthy ToM development improves social relationships with peers and produces greater desire to engage in prosocial behaviors. However, victimized individuals with highly-developed ToM may engage in increased aggression. Future longitudinal research is needed to investigate the functional outcomes resulting from ToM changes in adulthood.

References *Denotes studies included in the systematic review.

  • *Adrián, J. E., Clemente, R. A. & Villanueva, L. (2007). Mothers’ use of cognitive state verbs in picture-book reading and the development of children’s understanding of mind: A longitudinal study. Child Development, 78, 1052–1067. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01052.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Aschersleben, G., Hofer, T. & Jovanovic, B. (2008). The link between infant attention to goal-directed action and later theory of mind abilities. Developmental Science, 11, 862–868. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00736 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Astington, J. W. & Jenkins, J. M. (1999). A longitudinal study of the relation between language and theory-of-mind development. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1311–1320. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.35.5.1311 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Atkinson, L., Slade, L., Powell, D. & Levy, J. P. (2017). Theory of mind in emerging reading comprehension: A longitudinal study of early indirect and direct effects. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 164, 225–238. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.04.007 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Austin, G., Bondü, R. & Elsner, B. (2017). Longitudinal relations between children’s cognitive and affective theory of mind with reactive and proactive aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 43, 440–449. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21702 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Austin, G., Groppe, K. & Elsner, B. (2014). The reciprocal relationship between executive function and theory of mind in middle childhood: A 1-year longitudinal perspective. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00655 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baltes, P. B., Lindenberger, U. & Staudinger, U. M. (2006). Life span theory in developmental psychology. In W. DamonR. M. LernerEds., Handbook of child psychology, theoretical models of human development (6th ed., pp. 569–664). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • *Banerjee, R., Watling, D. & Caputi, M. (2011). Peer relations and the understanding of faux pas: Longitudinal evidence for bidirectional associations. Child Development, 82, 1887–1905. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01669.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). The autistic child’s theory of mind: A case of specific developmental delay. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 30, 285–297. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1989.tb00241.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bernstein, D. M., Coolin, A., Fischer, A., Thornton, W. L. & Sommerville, J. A. (2017). False-belief reasoning from 3 to 92 years of age. PLoS One, 12, e0185345. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0185345 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bora, E., Walterfang, M. & Velakoulis, D. (2015). Theory of mind in behavioural-variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: A meta-analysis. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 86, 714–719. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp-2014-309445 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Bosacki, S. L. (2013). A longitudinal study of children’s theory of mind, self-concept, and perceptions of humor in self and other. Social Behavior and Personality, 41, 663–673. https://doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2013.41.4.663 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brinck, I. (2004). The pragmatics of imperative and declarative pointing. Cognitive Science Quarterly, 3, 429–446. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • *Brooks, R. & Meltzoff, A. N. (2015). Connecting the dots from infancy to childhood: A longitudinal study connecting gaze following, language, and explicit theory of mind. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 130, 67–78. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.09.010 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Brown, J. R. & Dunn, J. (1991). “You can cry, mum”: The social and developmental implications of talk about internal states. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 237–256. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.1991.tb00874.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Caputi, M., Lecce, S., Pagnin, A. & Banerjee, R. (2012). Longitudinal effects of theory of mind on later peer relations: The role of prosocial behavior. Developmental Psychology, 48, 257–270. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025402 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Carlson, S. M., Mandell, D. J. & Williams, L. (2004). Executive function and theory of mind: Stability and prediction from ages 2 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 40, 1105–1122. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1105 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Centifanti, L. M., Meins, E. & Fernyhough, C. (2016). Callous-unemotional traits and impulsivity: Distinct longitudinal relations with mind-mindedness and understanding of others. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57, 84–92. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12445 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Baird, G., Cox, A. & Drew, A. (2000). Testing joint attention, imitation, and play as infancy precursors to language and theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 15, 481–498. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(01)00037-5 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Colonnesi, C., Rieffe, C., Koops, W. & Perucchini, P. (2008). Precursors of a theory of mind: A longitudinal study. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 26, 561–577. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151008X285660 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *De Mulder, H. (2015). Developing communicative competence: A longitudinal study of the acquisition of mental state terms and indirect requests. Journal of Child Language, 42, 969–1005. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000914000543 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Denham, S. A. (1986). Social cognition, prosocial behavior, and emotion in preschoolers: Contextual validation. Child Development, 57, 194–201. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130651 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Denham, S. A., Zoller, D. & Couchoud, E. A. (1994). Socialization of preschoolers’ emotion understanding. Developmental Psychology, 30, 928–936. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.30.6.928 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *de Villiers, J. G. & Pyers, J. E. (2002). Complements to cognition: A longitudinal study of the relationship between complex syntax and false-belief-understanding. Cognitive Development, 17, 1037–1060. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(02)00073-4 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Devine, R. T. & Hughes, C. (2014). Relations between false belief understanding and executive function in early childhood: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 85, 1777–1794. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12237 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • *Devine, R. T., White, N., Ensor, R. & Hughes, C. (2016). Theory of mind in middle childhood: Longitudinal associations with executive function and social competence. Developmental Psychology, 52, 758–771. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000105 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Dore, R. A. & Lillard, A. S. (2015). Theory of mind and children’s engagement in fantasy worlds. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 34, 230–242. https://doi.org/10.1177/0276236614568631 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Dunn, J. (1995). Children as psychologists: The later correlates of individual differences in understanding of emotions and other minds. Cognition and Emotion, 9, 187–201. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699939508409008 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Dunn, J., Brown, J., Slomkowski, C., Tesla, C. & Youngblade, L. (1991). Young children’s understanding of other people’s feelings and beliefs: Individual differences and their antecedents. Child Development, 62, 1352–1366. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130811 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Duval, C., Piolino, P., Bejanin, A., Eustache, F. & Desgranges, B. (2011). Age effects on different components of theory of mind. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 627–642. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2010.10.025 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Eggum, N. D., Eisenberg, N., Kao, K., Spinrad, T. L., Bolnick, R., Hofer, C., … Fabricius, W. V. (2011). Emotion understanding, theory of mind, and prosocial orientation: Relations over time in early childhood. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 6, 4–16. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2010.536776 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Ensor, R. & Hughes, C. (2008). Content or connectedness? Mother-child talk and early social understanding. Child Development, 79, 201–216. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01120.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Farrant, B. M., Maybery, M. T. & Fletcher, J. (2012). Language, cognitive flexibility, and explicit false belief understanding: Longitudinal analysis in typical development and specific language impairment. Child Development, 83, 223–235. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01681.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Farrar, M. J. & Maag, L. (2002). Early language development and the emergence of a theory of mind. First Language, 22, 197–213. https://doi.org/10.1177/014272370202206504 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Fink, E., Begeer, S., Hunt, C. & de Rosnay, M. (2014). False-belief understanding and social preference over the first 2 years of school: A longitudinal study. Child Development, 85, 2389–2403. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12302 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • *Fink, E., Begeer, S., Peterson, C. C., Slaughter, V. & de Rosnay, M. (2015). Friendlessness and theory of mind: A prospective longitudinal study. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 33, 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12060 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Flynn, E., O’Malley, C. & Wood, D. (2004). A longitudinal, microgenetic study of the emergence of false belief understanding and inhibition skills. Developmental Science, 7, 103–115. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00326.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Fonagy, P. (1996). The significance of the development of metacognitive control over mental representations in parenting and infant development. Journal of Clinical Psychoanalysis, 5, 67–86. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • German, T. P. & Hehman, J. A. (2006). Representational and executive selection resources in “theory of mind”: Evidence from compromised belief-desire reasoning in old age. Cognition, 101, 129–152. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005.05.007 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Guajardo, N. R. & Cartwright, K. B. (2016). The contribution of theory of mind, counterfactual reasoning, and executive function to pre-readers’ language comprehension and later reading awareness and comprehension in elementary school. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 144, 27–45. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.11.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Henry, J. D., Phillips, L. H., Ruffman, T. & Bailey, P. E. (2013). A meta-analytic review of age differences in theory of mind. Psychology and Aging, 28, 826–839. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030677 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Hughes, C. (1998). Finding your marbles: Does preschoolers’ strategic behavior predict later understanding of mind? Developmental Psychology, 34, 1326–1339. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.34.6.1326 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Hughes, C. & Dunn, J. (1998). Understanding mind and emotion: Longitudinal associations with mental-state talk between young friends. Developmental Psychology, 34, 1026–1037. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.34.5.1026 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Hughes, C. & Dunn, J. (2002). “When I say a naughty word”. A longitudinal study of young children’s accounts of anger and sadness in themselves and close others. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20, 515–535. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151002760390837 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Hughes, C. & Ensor, R. (2007). Executive function and theory of mind: Predictive relations from ages 2 to 4. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1447–1459. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1447 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Hughes, C., Ensor, R. & Marks, A. (2010). Individual differences in false belief understanding are stable from 3 to 6 years of age and predict children’s mental state talk with school friends. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 96–112. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.07.012 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Jenkins, J. M., Turrell, S. L., Kogushi, Y., Lollis, S. & Ross, H. S. (2003). A longitudinal investigation of the dynamics of mental state talk in families. Child Development, 74, 905–920. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00575 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Kirk, E., Pine, K., Wheatley, L., Howlett, N., Schulz, J. & Fletcher, B. (2015). A longitudinal investigation of the relationship between maternal mind-mindedness and theory of mind. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 33, 434–445. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12104 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kokkinos, C. M., Voulgaridou, I., Mandrali, M. & Parousidou, C. (2016). Interactive links between relational aggression, theory of mind, and moral disengagement among early adolescents. Psychology in the Schools, 53, 253–269. https://doi.org/10.1002/pits.21902 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Kristen, S., Sodian, B., Licata, M., Thoermer, C. & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2012). The development of internal state language during the third year of life: A longitudinal parent report study. Infant and Child Development, 21, 634–645. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.1767 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Kristen, S., Sodian, B., Thoermer, C. & Perst, H. (2011). Infants’ joint attention skills predict toddlers’ emerging mental state language. Developmental Psychology, 47, 1207–1219. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024808 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Kuhnert, R., Begeer, S., Fink, E. & de Rosnay, M. (2017). Gender-differentiated effects of theory of mind, emotion understanding, and social preference on prosocial behavior development: A longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 15, 413–427. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.10.001 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • *Laranjo, J., Bernier, A., Meins, E. & Carlson, S. M. (2010). Early manifestations of children’s theory of mind: The roles of maternal mind-mindedness and infant security of attachment. Infancy, 15, 300–323. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7078.2009.00014.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Lecce, S., Caputi, M. & Hughes, C. (2011). Does sensitivity to criticism mediate the relationship between theory of mind and academic achievement? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110, 313–331. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2011.04.011 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Lecce, S., Caputi, M. & Pagnin, A. (2014). Long-term effect of theory of mind on school achievement: The role of sensitivity to criticism. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 11, 305–318. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2013.821944 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Leslie, A. M. & Polizzi, P. (1998). Inhibitory processing in the false belief task: Two conjectures. Developmental Science, 1, 247–253. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00038 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lev-Wiesel, R., Nuttman-Shwartz, O. & Sternberg, R. (2006). Peer rejection during adolescence: Psychological long-term effects – A brief report. Journal of Loss and Trauma, 11, 131–142. https://doi.org/10.1080/15325020500409200 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Licata, M., Kristen, S. & Sodian, B. (2016). Mother-child interaction as a cradle of theory of mind: The role of maternal emotional availability. Social Development, 25, 139–156. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12131 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Low, J., Apperly, I. A., Butterfill, S. A. & Rakoczy, H. (2016). Cognitive architecture of belief reasoning in children and adults: A primer on the two-systems account. Child Development Perspectives, 10, 184–189. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12183 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Mansfield, A. F. & Clinchy, B. M. (2002). Toward the integration of objectivity and subjectivity: Epistemological development from 10 to 16. New Ideas in Psychology, 20, 225–262. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0732-118X(02)00008-9 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Marcovitch, S., O’Brien, M., Calkins, S. D., Leerkes, E. M., Weaver, J. M. & Levine, D. W. (2015). A longitudinal assessment of the relation between executive function and Theory of Mind at 3, 4, and 5 years. Cognitive Development, 33, 40–55. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2014.07.001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mascaro, J. S., Rilling, J. K., Negi, L. T. & Raison, C. L. (2013). Compassion meditation enhances empathic accuracy and related neural activity. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8, 48–55. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nss095 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *McAlister, A. R. & Peterson, C. C. (2007). A longitudinal study of child siblings and theory of mind development. Cognitive Development, 22, 258–270. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2006.10.009 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *McAlister, A. R. & Peterson, C. C. (2013). Siblings, theory of mind, and executive functioning in children aged 3–6 years: New longitudinal evidence. Child Development, 84, 1442–1458. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12043 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Russell, J. & Clark-Carter, D. (1998). Security of attachment as a predictor of symbolic and mentalising abilities: A longitudinal study. Social Development, 7, 1–24. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00047 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Wainwright, R., Gupta, M. D., Fradley, E. & Tuckey, M. (2002). Maternal mind-mindedness and attachment security as predictors of theory of mind understanding. Child Development, 73, 1715–1726. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00501 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Miller, S. A. (2012). Theory of mind beyond the preschool years. New York, NY: Psychology Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A. & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Moore, C., Furrow, D., Chiasson, L. & Patriquin, M. (1994). Developmental relationships between production and comprehension of mental terms. First Language, 14, 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1177/014272379401404001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Müller, U., Liebermann-Finestone, D. P., Carpendale, J. M., Hammond, S. I. & Bibok, M. B. (2012). Knowing minds, controlling actions: The developmental relations between theory of mind and executive function from 2 to 4 years of age. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111, 331–348. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2011.08.014 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Nelson, P. B., Adamson, L. B. & Bakeman, R. (2008). Toddlers’ joint engagement experience facilitates preschoolers’ acquisition of theory of mind. Developmental Science, 11, 847–852. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00733.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Newton, A. M. & de Villiers, J. G. (2007). Thinking while talking: Adults fail nonverbal false-belief reasoning. Psychological Science, 18, 574–579. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01942.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Newton, P., Reddy, V. & Bull, R. (2000). Children’s everyday deception and performance on false-belief tasks. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 18, 297–317. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151000165706 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Olineck, K. M. & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2007). Imitation of intentional actions and internal state language in infancy predict preschool theory of mind skills. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4, 14–30. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405620601046931 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Olson, S. L., Lopez-Duran, N., Lunkenheimer, E. S., Chang, H. & Sameroff, A. J. (2011). Individual differences in the development of early peer aggression: Integrating contributions of self-regulation, theory of mind, and parenting. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 253–266. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579410000775 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Onishi, K. H. & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science, 308, 255–258. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1107621 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Pellegrini, A. D., Van Ryzin, M. J., Roseth, C., Bohn-Gettler, C., Dupuis, D., Hickey, M. & Peshkam, A. (2011). Behavioral and social cognitive processes in preschool children’s social dominance. Aggressive Behavior, 37, 248–257. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.20385 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Perner, J., Lang, B. & Kloo, D. (2002). Theory of mind and self-control: More than a common problem of inhibition. Child Development, 73, 752–767. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00436 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pons, F., Harris, P. L. & de Rosnay, M. (2004). Emotion comprehension between 3 and 11 years: Developmental periods and hierarchical organization. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 1, 127–152. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405620344000022 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Premack, D. & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 515–526. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00076512 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Prime, H., Plamondon, A., Pauker, S., Perlman, M. & Jenkins, J. M. (2016). Sibling cognitive sensitivity as a moderator of the relationship between sibship size and children’s theory of mind: A longitudinal analysis. Cognitive Development, 39, 93–102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2016.03.005 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rafetseder, E. & Perner, J. (2018). Belief and counterfactuality: A teleological theory of belief attribution. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 226, 110–121. https://doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/a000327 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • *Renouf, A., Brendgen, M., Parent, S., Vitaro, F., Zelazo, P. D., Boivin, M., … Séguin, J. R. (2009). Relations between theory of mind and indirect and physical aggression in kindergarten: Evidence of the moderating role of prosocial behaviors. Social Development, 19, 535–555. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2009.00552.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Renouf, A., Brendgen, M., Séguin, J. R., Vitaro, F., Boivin, M., Dionne, G., … Pérusse, D. (2010). Interactive links between theory of mind, peer victimization, and reactive and proactive aggression. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38, 1109–1123. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-010-9432-z First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Ruffman, T., Slade, L. & Crowe, E. (2002). The relation between children’s and mothers’ mental state language and theory-of-mind understanding. Child Development, 73, 734–751. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00435 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ruffman, T., Slade, L., Rowlandson, K., Rumsey, C. & Garnham, A. (2003). How language relates to belief, desire, and emotion understanding. Cognitive Development, 18, 139–158. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(03)00002-9 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schlaffke, L., Lissek, S., Lenz, M., Juckel, G., Schultz, T., Tegenthoff, M., … Brüne, M. (2015). Shared and nonshared neural networks of cognitive and affective theory of mind: A neuroimaging study using cartoon picture stories. Human Brain Mapping, 36, 29–39. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22610 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Shakoor, S., Jaffee, S. R., Bowes, L., Ouellet-Morin, I., Andreou, P., Happé, F., … Arseneault, L. (2012). A prospective longitudinal study of children’s theory of mind and adolescent involvement in bullying. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 254–261. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02488.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Simon, A. & Bock, O. (2016). Influence of divergent and convergent thinking on visuomotor adaptation in young and older adults. Human Movement Science, 46, 23–29. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2015.11.020 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Slade, L. & Ruffman, T. (2005). How language does (and does not) relate to theory of mind: A longitudinal study of syntax, semantics, working memory and false belief. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23, 117–141. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151004X21332 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Sodian, B. & Kristen-Antonow, S. (2015). Declarative joint attention as a foundation of theory of mind. Developmental Psychology, 51, 1190–1200. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000039 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Song, J., Volling, B. L., Lane, J. D. & Wellman, H. M. (2016). Aggression, sibling antagonism, and theory of mind during the first year of siblinghood: A developmental cascade model. Child Development, 87, 1250–1263. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12530 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Suddendorf, T. & Fletcher-Flinn, C. M. (1999). Children’s divergent thinking improves when they understand false beliefs. Creativity Research Journal, 12, 115–128. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326934crj1202_4 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Sung, J. & Hsu, H. (2014). Collaborative mother-toddler communication and theory of mind development at age 4. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 35, 381–391. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2014.06.001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Symons, D. K. & Clark, S. E. (2000). A longitudinal study of mother-child relationships and theory of mind in the preschool period. Social Development, 9, 3–23. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00108 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Symons, D. K., Fossum, K. M. & Collins, T. K. (2006). A longitudinal study of belief and desire state discourse during mother-child play and later false belief understanding. Social Development, 15, 676–691. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2006.00364.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Talerico, K. A., Evans, L. K. & Strumpf, N. E. (2002). Mental health correlates of aggression in nursing home residents with dementia. The Gerontologist, 42, 169–177. https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/42.2.169 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Tardif, T., So, C. W. C. & Kaciroti, N. (2007). Language and false belief: Evidence for general, not specific, effects in Cantonese-speaking preschoolers. Developmental Psychology, 43, 318–340. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.2.318 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Taumoepeau, M. & Ruffman, T. (2006). Mother and infant talk about mental states relates to desire language and emotion understanding. Child Development, 77, 465–481. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00882.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Thoermer, C., Sodian, B., Vuori, M., Perst, H. & Kristen, S. (2012). Continuity from an implicit to an explicit understanding of false belief from infancy to preschool age. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30, 172–187. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02067.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Watson, A., Painter, K. & Bornstein, M. (2001). Longitudinal relations between 2-year-olds’ language and 4-year-olds’ theory of mind. Journal of Cognition and Development, 2, 449–457. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327647JCD0204_5 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wellman, H. M., Cross, D. & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72, 655–684. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00304 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Wellman, H. M., Lane, J. D., LaBounty, J. & Olson, S. L. (2011). Observant, nonaggressive temperament predicts theory-of-mind development. Developmental Science, 14, 319–326. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00977.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Wellman, H. M., Phillips, A. T., Dunphy-Lelii, S. & LaLonde, N. (2004). Infant social attention predicts preschool social cognition. Developmental Science, 7, 283–288. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00347.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wimmer, H. & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103–128. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • *Youngblade, L. M. & Dunn, J. (1995). Individual differences in young children’s pretend play with mother and sibling: Links to relationships and understanding of other people’s feelings and beliefs. Child Development, 66, 1472–1492. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131658 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar