Exploring Residual Capacity
The Effectiveness of a Vibrotactile Warning During Increasing Levels of Mental Workload in Simulated Flight Tasks
Abstract
Abstract. Alarm systems may take advantage of the tactile modality for allocation of attentional resources during the performance of demanding tasks in complex environments. The purpose of this study was to determine the effectiveness of a tactile warning during increasing levels of mental workload in a primary task. Three simulated flight task conditions varying in mental workload were presented while an “on-thigh” vibrotactile warning was randomly assessed. Generally, there was a decrement in overall warning response performance when task workload increased, but this tendency faded and plateaued as the level of task workload progressed. The observed pattern indicates that vibrotactile warning signals may offer a plausible mode for conveying information during increasing levels of primary task workload.
References
2003). Investigation of alarm-related accidents and incidents in aviation. The International Journal of Aviation Psychology, 13(3), 249–268. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327108ijap1303_04
(2010). The problem state: A cognitive bottleneck in multitasking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(2), 363–382. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018106
(2007). A review of the mental workload literature (Technical Report No. 0704–0188). Toronto, Canada: Defense Research and Development
(2016). The role of cognitive and perceptual loads in inattentional deafness. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, 344. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00344
(2008). Behavioral evidence for task-dependent “what” versus “where” processing within and across modalities. Perception & Psychophysics, 70(1), 36–49. https://doi.org/10.3758/pp.70.1.36
(1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Academic Press.
(2006). Cockpit display of traffic information automated conflict alerting: Parameters to maximize effectiveness and minimize disruption in multi-task environments (Technical Report No. AHFD-05–22/NASA-05–9). Moffet Field, CA: NASA Ames Research Center.
(2006). Vibrotactile and visual threat cueing with high-G threat intercept in dynamic flight simulation. Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, 50(16), 1547–1551. https://doi.org/10.1177/154193120605001607
(2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03193146
(2007). Toward developing an approach for alerting drivers to the direction of a crash threat. Human Factors, 49(4), 710–720. https://doi.org/10.1518/001872007x215782
(2007). The cognitive and neural correlates of tactile consciousness: A multisensory perspective. Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 370–407. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2007.01.005
(2015). P300 event-related potential as an indicator of inattentional deafness? PLoS One, 10(2), e0118556. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118556
(1984). On the psychophysics of workload: Why bother with subjective measures? Human Factors, 26(5), 519–532. https://doi.org/10.1177/001872088402600504
(1986).
(Workload: An examination of the concept . In K. R. BoffL. KaufmanJ. P. ThomasEds., Handbook of perception and human performance (pp. 41–43). New York: John Wiley & Sons.2004). Haptic rendering–beyond visual computing–Deriving haptic design guidelines from human physiological, psychophysical and neurological foundations. IEEE Computer Graphics and Applications, 24(2), 33–39. https://doi.org/10.1109/mcg.2004.1274059
(2006). Assessing the effectiveness of “intuitive” vibrotactile warning signals in preventing front-to-rear-end collisions in a driving simulator. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 38(5), 988–996. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2006.04.002
(1993). Workload transition: Implications for individual and team performance. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
(1988).
(Measuring mental workload: Problems, progress, and promise . In P. HancockN. MeshkatiEds., Human mental workload (pp. 5–39). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: North-Holland. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0166-4115(08)62381-x2008). Tactile displays: Guidance for their design and application. Human Factors, 50(1), 90–111. https://doi.org/10.1518/001872008x250638
(2012). Cross-modal distraction by deviance: Functional similarities between the auditory and tactile modalities. Experimental Psychology, 59(6), 355–363. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000164
(1989). Operator workload: Comprehensive review and evaluation of operator workload methodologies (Technical Report No. 2075-3). Willow Grove, PA: U.S. Army Research Institute for the Behavioral and Social Sciences. https://doi.org/10.21236/ada212879
(1967). Where is capacity limited? A survey and a model. Acta Psychologica, 27, 84–92.
(2003).
(Multimodal interfaces . In J. JackoA. SearsEds., Handbook of human-computer interaction (pp. 286–304). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.1999).
(Using signal detection theory and Bayesian analysis to design parameters for automated alarm systems . In M. ScerboM. MoulouaEds., Automation Technology and Human Performance: Current Research and Trends (pp. 63–67). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.2001).
(Adaptive control and mental workload . In P. A. HancockP. A. DesmondEds., Stress, workload and fatigue: Human factors in transportation (pp. 305–320). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.2011). A behavioral study of distraction by vibrotactile novelty. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37(4), 1134–1139. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021931
(2007). PsychoPy–Psychophysics software in Python. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 162(1–2). (8–13). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2006.11.017
(2005). A multitasking general executive for compound continuous tasks. Cognitive Science, 29(3), 457–492. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog0000_19
(2006). Modeling driver behavior in a cognitive architecture. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 48(2), 362–380. https://doi.org/10.1518/001872006777724417
(2008). Threaded cognition: An integrated theory of concurrent multitasking. Psychological Review, 115(1), 101–130. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.115.1.101
(2012). A comparison of ‘on-thigh’ vibrotactile, combined visual-vibrotactile, and visual-only alerting systems for the cockpit under visually demanding conditions. Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, 56(1), 1644–1648. https://doi.org/10.1177/1071181312561329
(2011). Vibrotactile “on-thigh” alerting system in the cockpit. Human Factors, 53(2), 118–131. https://doi.org/10.1177/0018720811403139
(2008). Capturing spatial attention with multisensory cues. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 398–403. https://doi.org/10.3758/pbr.15.2.398
(2002).
(Multimodal information presentation in support of human-automation communication and coordination . In E. SalasEd., Advances in human performance and cognitive engineering research (pp. 13–36). New York: JAI Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1479-3601(02)02004-02006). Multimodal information presentation: Design guidance and research challenges. International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, 36(5), 439–445. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ergon.2006.01.007
(1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: Detection, search, and attention. Psychological Review, 84(1), 1–66. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.84.1.1
(2008). A comparison of tactile, visual, and auditory warnings for rear-end collision prevention in simulated driving. Human Factors, 50(2), 264–275. https://doi.org/10.1518/001872008X250674
(1999). Good vibrations: Tactile feedback in support of attention allocation and human-automation coordination in event-driven domains. Human Factors, 41(4), 543–552. https://doi.org/10.1518/001872099779656716
(1997). Cross-modal links in attention between audition, vision, and touch: Implications for interface design. International Journal of Cognitive Ergonomics, 1(4), 351–373. Retrieved from http://psycnet.apa.org/record/1999-10777-005
(2003). Production compilation: A simple mechanism to model complex skill acquisition. Human Factors, 45(1), 61–76. https://doi.org/10.1518/hfes.45.1.61.27224
(2005).
(Perceiving ordinal data haptically under workload . In ACMEds., Proceedings of the 7th international conference on Multimodal Interfaces (pp. 317–324). New York, NY: ACM. https://doi.org/10.1145/1088463.10885172006). Throughput: A simple performance index with desirable characteristics. Behavior Research Methods, 38(4), 569–573. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193886
(1995). False alarms in naval aircraft: A review of naval safety center mishap data (Special Report No 95–003). Orlando, FL: Naval Air Warfare Center Training Systems Division.
(2018). Further tests of the utility of integrated speed-accuracy measures in task switching. Journal of Cognition, 1(1), 1–16. https://doi.org/10.5334/joc.6
(2006). A tactile cockpit instrument supports the control of self-motion during spatial disorientation. Human Factors, 48(2), 219–228. https://doi.org/10.1518/001872006777724435
(2005). Waypoint navigation with a vibrotactile waist belt. ACM Transactions on Applied Perception, 2(2), 106–117. https://doi.org/10.1145/1060581.1060585
(2002, April). Tactile torso display as countermeasure to reduce night vision goggles induced drift. Paper presented at the meeting of Spatial disorientation in military vehicles: Causes, consequences and cures, NATO Research and Technology Organization, Neuilly-sur Seine, France. https://doi.org/10.1007/3-540-44589-7_19
(2004).
(Waypoint navigation on land: Different ways of coding distance to the next waypoint . In M. BussM. FritschiH. EsenEds., Proceedings of EuroHaptics 2004 (pp. 160–165). Munich, Germany: Technische Universität München.2001).
(Tactile information presentation in the cockpit . In S. BrewsterR. Murray-SmithEds., Haptic human-computer interaction (pp. 174–181). Berlin, Germany: Springer.2015a). Audition and vision share spatial attentional resources, yet attentional load does not disrupt audiovisual integration. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(July), 1–12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01084
(2015b). Vision and haptics share spatial attentional resources and visuotactile integration is not affected by high attentional load. Multisensory Research, 28, 371–392. https://doi.org/10.1163/22134808-00002482
(2016) Attentional resource allocation in visuotactile processing depends on the task, but optimal visuotactile integration does not depend on attentional resources. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 10(March), 1–13. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnint.2016.00013
(2017). Is attentional resource allocation across sensory modalities task-dependent? Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 13(1), 83–96. https://doi.org/10.5709/acp-0209-2
(2002). Multiple resources and performance prediction. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics Science, 3(2), 159–177. https://doi.org/10.1080/14639220210123806
(2007). Dual-task performance consequences of imperfect alerting associated with a cockpit display of traffic information. Human Factors, 49(5), 839–850. https://doi.org/10.1518/001872007X230217
(1954). On the vibrational sensitivity in different regions of the body surface. Acta Physiologica Scandinavica, 31, 285–289. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1748-1716.1954.tb01139.x
(2015). State of science: mental workload in ergonomics. Ergonomics, 58(1), 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1080/00140139.2014.956151
(