Skip to main content
Kommentierte Übersichtsarbeit

Blickbewegungen beim Lesen, Leseentwicklung und Legasthenie

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1024/2235-0977/a000019

Zusammenfassung: In den letzten Jahrzehnten hat sich die Messung und Analyse von Blickbewegungen mehr und mehr zur einer Standardmethode für die Untersuchung des Lesens und der Leseentwicklung im Kindesalter entwickelt. Im vorliegenden Artikel werden zunächst einige grundlegende Erkenntnisse zur Dynamik des Lesens aus der Sicht der Blickbewegungsforschung skizziert. Auf dieser Grundlage erfolgt eine Darstellung wichtiger Aspekte der normalen und gestörten Leseentwicklung. Dabei wird vertiefend auf Befunde zu Blickbewegungen von Kindern mit Legasthenie eingegangen, die zum Teil widersprüchlich sind und in der Literatur kontrovers diskutiert werden. Hieraus ergeben sich Empfehlungen für die weitere Forschung bezüglich der Identifikation visuomotorischer Defizite, der Informationsaufnahme innerhalb der Blickspanne sowie die Verarbeitung phonologischer und räumlicher Information. Abschließend wird eine leseähnliche Scanningaufgabe vorgestellt, mit der die nichtsprachlichen Anforderungen des Lesens gut approximiert werden können. Damit kann die Frage untersucht werden, ob abweichende Blickbewegungen bei Legasthenie auch durch Faktoren wie visuelle Informationsverarbeitung und Aufmerksamkeit, Blicksteuerung oder räumliche Navigation mitverursacht sein können.


Extended abstract

Eye Movement in Reading, Reading Development and Developmental Dyslexia

Background: Over the last few decades, the monitoring of eye movements has become a standard methodology in the scientific study of reading and reading development. There is now ample evidence demonstrating that the direction and sequence of eye movements during reading is co-determined by both basic visual and visuomotor constraints and cognitive processing demands. Most importantly, the duration and number of fixations is closely related to linguistic processing on the level of letters, words and sentences. Consequently, word viewing time measures present valid indicators of moment-to-moment processing during reading. This can serve as a solid base for research into reading development and reading problems such as developmental dyslexia.

Aims: This article has three related aims. First, we present a brief outline of basic trends in reading development from the perspective of eye movement research, based on data collected with elementary school level readers. Second, we discuss the use of eye movement methodology to study developmental dyslexia with respect to both eye movements in non-reading tasks and oculomotor analyses of reading itself. Finally, on the basis of the present state of the art in research on normal development, a number of suggestions are made on how to utilize eye tracking methodology to advance our understanding of developmental dyslexia.

Results: We present and discuss results from a number of studies demonstrating the utility of eye movements in examining developmental trajectories of both silent and oral reading. Major findings include a trend towards more and more automatic and less sequential word processing that is clearly reflected in word based fixation patterns. Particularly interesting is the fact that very young readers appear to spend a large amount of reading time with post-lexical, sentence level integration of meaning. On the other hand it appears that in most children the basic machinery of eye movement control is in place quite early. A comparison of silent and oral reading points to a very interesting difference between reading in transparent vs. intransparent orthographies. English readers not only need more time to read equivalent sentences, but the difference between reading aloud and silently is much more pronounced. Looking at new ways to examine eye movements in struggling readers we suggest a new methodology aimed at approximating the non-linguistic processing demands of continuous reading. Results from two validation studies indicate that important aspects of visual processing of eye movement control in this reading-like string scanning task are functionally equivalent to normal reading in both adults and children.

Discussion: We argue that despite major progress we are still lacking a comprehensive picture of the directions and determinants of development in normal reading. The best way of addressing these complex and challenging issues will be in terms of well controlled and comprehensive longitudinal research projects. In our view a key condition for success in this endeavor will be the combination of oculomotor reading measures with psychometric assessments of reading ability and cognitive component skills. Based on recent advances in eye tracking theory and methodology, we are considering a number of directions in future research on developmental dyslexia. These issues include the identification of visuomotor deficits, extrafoveal information acquisition within the perceptual span, the processing of phonological information as well as spatial navigation and memory. Central to this proposed research strategy is the use of our reading-like string scanning task. We expect this new research tool to help answering the question whether deviations of eye movements in struggling readers reflect non-linguistic processing deficits that might play a role in the etiology of developmental dyslexia.

Literatur

  • Becker, W. , Jürgens, R. (1979). An analysis of the saccadic system by means of double step stimuli. Vision Research, 19, 967– 984. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Biscaldi, M. , Fischer, B. , Hartnegg, K. , Gutjahr, G. (2000). Voluntary saccade control in dyslexia. Perception, 29, 509– 521. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bosse, M. L. , Tainturier, M. J. , Valdois, S. (2007). Developmental dyslexia: the visual attention span deficit hypothesis. Cognition, 104:2, 198– 230. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Buswell, G. T. (1922). Fundamental reading habits, a study of their development. Chicago: Chicago University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Crawford, T. J. , Higham, S. (2001). Dyslexia and the centre-of-gravity effect. Experimental Brain Research, 137: 1, 122– 126. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Castles, A. , Coltheart, M. (1993). Varieties of developmental dyslexia. Cognititon, 47, 149– 180. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ciuffreda, K. , Tannen, B. (1995). Eye movement basics for the clinician. Mosby: St. Louis. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Denckla, M. B. , Rudel, R. (1974). Rapid “automatized” naming of pictured objects, colors, letters, and numbers by normal children. Cortex, 10 (2), 186– 202. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Deubel, H. , O’Regan, K. , Radach, R. (2000). Attention, information processing and eye movement control. In Kennedy, A., Radach, R., Heller, D. & Pynte, J. (Eds). Reading as a Perceptual Process. Elsevier, Oxford. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dusek, W. , Pierscionek, B. K. , McClelland, J. F. (2010). A survey of visual function in an Austrian population of school-age children with reading and writing difficulties. BMC Ophtalmology, 10: 16. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Dürrwächter, U. , Sokolov, A. N. , Reinhard, J. , Klosinski, G. , Trauzettel-Klosinski, S. (2010). Word length and word frequency affect eye movements in dyslexic children reading in a regular (German) orthography. Annals of Dyslexia, 60, 86– 101. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Engbert, R. , Nuthmann, A. , Richter, E. M. , Kliegl, R. (2005). SWIFT: a dynamical model of saccade generation during reading. Psychological Review, 112, 777– 813. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Evans, B. J. W. (2001). Dyslexia and Vision, Whurr, London, 2001 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Facoetti, A, , Molteni, M. (2000). The gradient of visual attention in developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 39, 352– 7. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Findlay, J. M. , Walker, R. (1999). A model of saccade generation based on parallel processing and competitive inhibition. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 22, 661– 721. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fischer, B. , Weber, H. (1990). Saccadic reaction times of dyslexic and age-matched normal subjects. Perception, 19, 805– 818. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fischer, B. , Hartnegg, K. (2000). Effects of visual training on saccade control in dyslexia. Perception, 29, 531– 542. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Geiger, G. , Lettvin, J. Y. (1987). Peripheral vision in persons with dyslexia. New England Journal of Medicine, 316, 1238– 1243. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Grainger, J. (2008). Cracking the orthographic code: An introduction. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23 (1), 1– 35. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Häikiö, T. , Bertram, R. , Hyönä, J. , Niemi, P. (2009). Development of the letter identity span in reading: Evidence from the eye movement moving window paradigm. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102, 167– 181. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hawelka, St. , Gagl, B. , Wimmer, H. (2010). A dual-route perspective on eye movements of dyslexic readers. Cognition, 115, 367– 379. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Heller, D. , Radach, R. (1999). Eye movements in reading: Are two eyes better than one? In Becker, W., Deubel, H. & Mergner, T. (Eds). Current Oculomotor Research: Physiological and Psychological Aspects. Plenum Publishers , New York. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Heim, S. , Tschierse, J. , Amunts, K. , Wilms, M. , Vossel, S. , Willmes, K. et al. (2008). Cognitive subtypes of dyslexia. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentals, 68, 73– 82. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hofmeister, J. , Heller, D. , Radach, R. (1999). The return sweep in reading. In Becker, W., Deubel, H. & Mergner, T. (Eds). Current Oculomotor Research: Physiological and Psychological Aspects. New York, Plenum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Huestegge, L. (2010). Effects of vowel length on gaze durations in silent and oral reading. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 3, 1– 18. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Huestegge, L. , Radach, R. , Corbic, D. , Huestegge, S. M. (2009). Oculomotor and Linguistic Determinants of Reading Development: A Longitudinal Study. Vision Research, 49, 2948– 2959. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hutzler, F., Kronbichler, M., Jacobs, A. M., Wimmer, H. (2006). Perhaps correlational but not causal: no effect of dyslexic readers’ magnocellular system on their eye movements during reading. Neuropsychologia, 44, 637– 48. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hutzler, F. , Wimmer, H. (2004). Eye movements of dyslexic children when reading in a regular orthography. Brain and Language, 89 (1), 235– 242. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hyönä, J. , Olson, R. K. (1995). Eye fixation patterns among dyslexic and normal readers: Effects of word length and word frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 6, 1430– 1440. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Inhoff, A. W. , Connine, C. , Eiter, B. , Radach, R. , Heller, D. (2004). Phonological representations of words in working memory during sentence reading. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 320– 325. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Inhoff, A. W. , Connine, C. , Radach, R. (2002). A Contingent Speech Technique in Eye Movement Research on Reading. Behavior Research Methods, 34. 471– 80. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Inhoff, A. W. , Solomon, M. , Radach, R. , Seymour, B. (2011). Temporal dynamics of the eye voice span and eye movement control during oral reading. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 23, 543– 558. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Inhoff, A. W. , Radach, R. (1998). Definition and Computation of Oculomotor Measures in the Study of Cognitive Processes. In Underwood, G. (Ed.). Eye guidance in reading and scene perception. Elsevier, Oxford. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Inhoff, A. W. , Topolski, R. (1994). Use of phonological codes during eye fixations in reading and on-line delayed naming tasks. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 689– 713. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Inhoff, A. W. , Weger, U. W. , Radach, R. (2005). Sources of information for the programming of short- and long-range regressions during reading. In Underwood, G. (Ed.). Cognitive Processes in Eye Guidance. Oxford University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jacobs, A. M. , Grainger, J. (1994). Models of visual word recognition: Sampling the state of the art. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 1311– 1334. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jacobs, A. M. , Heller, D. , Nazir, T. A. (1992). Möglichkeiten einer experimentellen Dyslexieforschung auf der Basis der aktuellen Lesepsychologie. Schweizerische Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 51 (1), 26– 42. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Jainta, S., Kapoula, Z. (2011). Dyslexic children are confronted with unstable binocular fixation while reading. PLoS One, 6 (4), 6: e18694. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kennedy, A. , Brooks, R. , Flynn, L.-A. , Prophet, C. (2003). The reader’s spatial code. In Hyönä, J., Radach, R. & Deubel, H. (Eds.). The Mind’s Eyes: Cognitive and Applied Aspects of Eye Movements. Elsevier Science, Oxford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kirkby, J. A. , Blythe, H. I. , Drieghe, D. , Liversedge, S. P. (2011). Reading text increases binocular disparity in dyslexic children. PLoS One. 6, Epub 2011 Nov 4. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Klein, R. , Berry, G. , Briand, K. , D’Entremont, B. , Farmer, M. (1990). Letter identification declines with increasing retinal eccentricity at the same rate for normal and dyslexic readers. Perception and Psychophysics, 47: 6, 601– 606. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kliegl, R., Grabner, E., Rolfs, M., Engbert, R. (2004). Length, frequency, and predictability effects of words on eye movements in reading. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 16, 262– 284. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kliegl, R., Olson, R. K. , Davidson, B. J. (1983). On problems of unconfounding perceptual and language processes. In K. Rayner (Ed.). Eye Movements in Reading and Perceptual and Language Processes. Academic Press, New York. 333– 343. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kliegl, R. , Nuthmann, A. , Engbert, R. (2006). Tracking the mind during reading: The influence of past, present, and future words on fixation durations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135, 12– 35. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Krügel, A. , Engbert, R. (2010). On the launch-site effect for skipped words during reading. Vision Research, 50, 1532– 1539. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lee, Y. , Binder, K. , Kim, J. , Pollatsek, A. , Rayner, K. (1999). Activation of phonological codes during eye fixations in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Human Perception and Performance, 25, 948– 964. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lorusso, M. L. , Facoetti, A. , Pesenti, S. , Cattaneo, C. , Molteni, M. , Geiger, G. (2004). Wider recognition in peripheral vision common to different subtypes of dyslexia. Vision Research, 44: 20, 2413– 2424. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McConkie, G. W. , Zola, D. , Grimes, J. , Kerr, P. W. , Bryant, N. R. , Wolff, P. M. (1991). Children’s eye movements during reading. In J.F. Stein (Ed.). Vision and visual dyslexia, (pp. 251 – 262). London: Macmillian Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • McConkie, G. W. , Kerr, P. W. , Reddix, M. D. , Zola, D. , Jacobs, A. M. (1989). Eye movement control during reading: II. Frequency of refixating a word. Perception & Psychophysics, 46, 245– 253. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McConkie, G. W. , Kerr, P. W. , Reddix, M. D. , Zola, D. (1988). Eye movement control during reading: I. The location of initial eye fixations in words. Vision research, 28, 1107– 1118. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Moll, K. , Wallner, R. , Landerl, K. (2012). Kognitive Korrelate der Lese-, Leserechtschreib und der Rechtschreibstörung. Lernen und Lernstörungen, 1, 7– 20. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Nuthmann, A. , Engbert, R. (2009). Mindless reading revisited: an analysis based on the SWIFT model of eye-movement control. Vision Research, 49, 322– 36. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nuthmann, A. , Kliegl, R. (2009). An examination of binocular reading fixations based on sentence corpus data. Journal of Vision, 29, 1– 28. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Olson, R. K. , Conners, F. A. , Rack, J. P. (1991). Eye movements in dyslexic and normal readers. In J.F. Stein (Ed.). Vision and visual dyslexia, (pp. 243 – 250). London: Macmillian Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Olson, R. K. , Kliegl, R. , Davidson, B. J. (1983). Dyslexic and normal readers’eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9, 816– 825. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • O’Regan, J. K. (1992). Optimal viewing position in words and the strategy-tactics theory of eye movements in reading. In K. Rayner (Ed.). Eye movements and visual cognition: Scene perception and reading, (pp. 333 – 354). New York: Springer. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Pavlidis, G. T. (1981). Do eye movements hold the key to dyslexia?. Neuropsychologia, 19, 57– 64. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pollatsek, A., Lesch, M., Morris, R. K. , Rayner, K. (1992). Phonological codes are used in integrating information across saccades in word identification and reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 18, 148– 162. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Prado, C. , Dubois, M. , Valdois, S. (2007). The eye movements of dyslexic children during reading and visual search: impact of the visual attention span. Vision Research, 47, 2521– 2530. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pirozzolo, F. J. (1983). Eye movements and reading disability. In K. Rayner (Ed.). Eye Movements in reading: Perceptual and language processes (pp. 499 – 509). Academic Press, New York. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Radach, R. (1994). Elementare Probleme der Informationsaufnahme beim Lesen. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 202, 37– 62. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Radach, R. , Heller, D. (2000). Relations between Spatial and Temporal Aspects of Eye Movement Control. In A. Kennedy, R. Radach, D. Heller and J. Pynte (Eds.). Reading as a Perceptual Process (pp. 165 – 192) Elsevier, Oxford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Radach, R. , Heller, D. , Huestegge, L. (2002). Blickbewegungen beim Lesen: neueste Entwicklungen und Ansatzpunkte für die Legasthenieforschung. In Legasthenie und Rechenstörung. Aktuelle Ergebnisse aus Praxis und Forschung. Winkler-Verlag. Bochum, 2002. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Radach, R. , Huestegge, L. , Reilly, R. (2008). The role of top down factors in local eye movement control during reading. Psychological Research, 72, 675– 688. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Radach, R. ,  Inhoff, A. W. , Heller, D. (2004). Orthographic regularity gradually modulates saccade amplitudes in reading. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 16, 27– 51. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Radach, R. , McConkie, G. W. (1998). Determinants of fixation positions in words during reading. In Underwood, G. (Ed.). Eye guidance in reading and scene perception. Elsevier, Oxford. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Radach, R. , Reilly, R. , Inhoff, A. W. (2007). Models of oculomotor control in reading: Towards a theoretical foundation of current debates. In R. van Gompel, M. Fischer, W. Murray, & R. Hill: Eye movements: A window on mind and brain. Elsevier: Oxford, pp. 237 – 270. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Radach, R. , Kennedy, A. (2004). Theoretical perspectives on eye movements in reading: Past controversies, current deficits and an agenda for future research. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 16, 3– 26. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Radach, R. , Schmitten, C. , Glover, L. , Huestegge, L. (2009). How children read for comprehension: Eye movements in developing readers. In Wagner, R. K., Schatschneider, C., & Phythian-Sence, C. (Eds.) Beyond Decoding: The Bilogical and Behavioral Foundations of Reading Comprehension.. Guildford Press. New York City. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Radach, R. , Wohlgemuth, Z. , Vorstius, C. , Günther, T. (2009). Mindless reading? A new paradigm for visuomotor control and distributed processing during reading-like scanning.. 15th European Conference on Eye Movements.. Southampton, England. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Radach, R. , Vorstius, C. (2011). Visual processing and spatial memory co-determine the accuracy of short and long range regressions in reading. 16th European Conference on Eye Movements. Southampton, England. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ramus, F. , Rosen, S. , Dakin, S. C. , Day, B. L. , Castellote, J. M. , White, S. , Frith, U. (2003). Theories of dyslexia: insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults. Brain, 126, 841– 65. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ram-Tsur, R. , Faust, M. , Caspi, A. , Gordon, C. R. , Zivotofsky, A. Z. (2006). Evidence for ocular-motor deficits in developmental dyslexia: Application of the Double-Step Paradigm. Investigative Ophthalmology Visual Science, 47, 4401– 4409. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rayner, K. (2009). Eye movements and attention in reading, scene perception, and visual search. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62:8, 1457– 1506. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rayner, K. (1986). Eye movements and the perceptual span in beginning and skilled readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 41, 211– 236. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rayner, K. , Fischer, M. H. (1996). Mindless reading revisited: eye movements during reading and scanning are different. Perception & Psychophysics, 58, 734– 747. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rayner, K. , Pollatsek, A. , Ashby, J. , Clifton, C. (2011). Psychology of reading. Second Edition. Taylor & Francis, New York First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Reichle, E. D. , Rayner, K. , Pollatsek, A. (2003). The E-Z Reader model of eye movement control in reading: Comparisons to other models. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 26, 445– 476. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Reilly, R. , Radach, R. (2006). Some empirical tests of an interactive activation model of eye movement control in reading. Cognitive Systems Research, 7, 34– 55. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ruland, A. , Willmes, K. , Günther, T. (2012). Zusammenhang zwischen Aufmerksamkeitsdefiziten und Lese- Rechtschreibschwäche. Kindheit und Entwicklung, 21 (1), 57– 63. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Schattka, K. , Radach, R. , Huber, W. (2010). Eye movement correlates of acquired central dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 48, 2959– 2973. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schotter, E. R. , Angele, B. , Rayner, K. (2012). Parafoveal processing in reading. Attention, Perception and Psychophysics, 74, 5– 35. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sprenger-Charolles, L. (2011). Dyslexia subtypes in languages differing in orthographic transparency: English, French and Spanish. Escritos de Psicología, 4 (2), 5– 16. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Stein, J. . The magnocellular theory of developmental dyslexia. Dyslexia. 2001, 7, 12– 36. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Stenneken, P. , Egetemeir, J. , Schulte-Körne, G. , Müller, H.-J. , Werner, X. , Schneider, W. X. , Finke, K. (2011). Slow perceptual processing at the core of developmental dyslexia: A parameter-based assessment of visual attention. Neuropsychologia, 49, 3454– 3465. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tang, S. , Reilly, R. G. , Vorstius, C. (2011). EyeMap: a software system for visualizing and analyzing eye movement data in reading. Behavioral Research Methods, (epub ahead of print). First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Taylor, S. E. , Frackenpohl, H. , Petee, J. L. (1960). Grade level norms for the components of the fundamental reading skill. E.D.L.. Research and Information Bulletin, 3. Huntington, NY: Educational Developmental Labs, Inc.. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Trauzettel-Klosinski, S. , Koitzsch, A. M. , Dürrwächter, U. , Sokolov, A. N. , Reinhard, J. , Klosinski, G. (2010). Eye movements in German-speaking children with and without dyslexia when reading aloud. Acta Ophthalmologica, 88, 681– 691. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Underwood, N. R. , Zola, D. (1986). The span of letter recognition of good and poor readers. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 6– 19. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vellutino, F. R. , Fletcher, J. M. , Snowling, M. J. , Scanlon, D. M. (2004). Specfic reading disability (dyslexia): what have we learned in the past four decades?. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 45, 2– 40. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vitu, F. , O’Regan, J. K. , Inhoff, A. W. , Topolski, R. (1995). Mindless reading: eye-movement characteristics are similar in scanning letter strings and reading texts. Perception & Psychophysics, 57, 352– 364. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wade, N. J. , Tatler, B. W. , Heller, D. (2003). Dodge-ing the issue: Dodge, Javal, Hering, and the measurement of saccades in eye-movement research. Perception, 32 (7), 793– 804. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wagner, R. K. , Schatschneider, C. , Phythian-Sence, C. (Eds., 2009). Beyond Decoding: The Bilogical and Behavioral Foundations of Reading Comprehension. Guildford Press. New York City. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Willcutt, E. G. , Pennigton, B. F. , Olson, R. K. , Hulslander, J. (2005). Neuropsychological Analyses of Comorbidity Between Reading Disability and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: In Search of the Common Deficit. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27, 35– 78. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wimmer, H. , Goswami, U. (1994). The influence of orthographic consistency on reading development: Word recognition in English and German children. Cognition, 51, 91– 103. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wimmer, H. , Schurz, M. (2010). Dyslexia in regular orthographies: manifestation and causation. Dyslexia, 16, 283– 99. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wolf, M. , Bowers, P. G. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 93, 415– 438. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Wotschack, C. , Kliegl, R. (2011). Reading strategy modulates parafoveal-on-foveal effects in sentence reading. Quartely Journal of Experimental Psychology. [Sept. 20, Epub ahead of print] First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ziegler, J. C. , Pech-Georgel, C. , Dufau, S. , Grainger, J. (2010). Rapid processing of letters, digits and symbols: what purely visual-attentional deficit in developmental dyslexia?. Developmental Sciences, 13: 4, F8– F14. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ziegler, J. C. , Perry, C. , Ma-Wyatt, A. , Ladner, D. , Schulte-Körne, G. (2003). Developmental dyslexia in different languages: Language-specific or universal? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 286, 169– 193. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar