Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1026/0942-5403/a000163

Um die Beziehungen zwischen den rasch wachsenden Fähigkeiten zur Selbstregulation durch Exekutive Funktionen (EF) und dem Emotionswissen einerseits und Veränderungen bei Aufmerksamkeitsproblemen andererseits auszuleuchten, wurden 261 3- bis 6-jährige Kinder einzeln zu diesen Konstrukten befragt und ihre Erzieherinnen im Kindergarten zu zwei Messzeitpunkten im Abstand von etwa 14 Monaten um Einschätzungen eventueller Aufmerksamkeitsprobleme gebeten. Hierarchische Regressionsanalysen weisen darauf hin, dass die beiden Testverfahren zur Messung von Arbeitsgedächtnis und Inhibition bei den EF und das Emotionswissen jeweils zur Vorhersage der Veränderungen der Aufmerksamkeitsprobleme beitrugen, auch wenn bekannte Prädiktoren wie Geschlecht, sozioökonomischer Status und Sprachverständnis kontrolliert worden waren. Wurden Emotionswissen und EF in ein gemeinsames Modell einbezogen, dann klärte das Emotionswissen über die beiden Varianten der EF hinaus zusätzliche Varianz bei den Veränderungen der Aufmerksamkeitsprobleme auf. Diskutiert werden die Wege, durch die das Emotionswissen die Aufmerksamkeitslenkung beeinflusst.


Emotion Knowledge, Executive Functions, and Changes in Attention Problems in Kindergarten-Age Children

The aim of this study was to examine the relationships between the rapidly growing abilities of preschool children to self-regulate by means of executive functions (EF) and emotion knowledge, on one hand, and changes in attention problems, on the other hand. For this, 261 3- to 6-year-olds were tested on these constructs and their preschool and kindergarten teachers were asked to rate their attention problems at two time points that were about 14 months apart. Hierarchical regressions indicate that emotion knowledge and both measures of working memory and inhibition as parts of EF individually contributed to the prediction of change in attention problems, after known predictors such as the children’s sex, their family’s socioeconomic status, and their receptive language skills were controlled for. When emotion knowledge and EF were included in a joint model, emotion knowledge explained the additional variance of the changes in attention problems above and beyond both measures of EF. Here, we discuss ways in which emotion knowledge influences attention control.

Literatur

  • Barkley, R. A. (2006). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A handbook for diagnosis and treatment (3rd ed.). New York: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Blankson, A. N., O’Brien, M. O., Leerkes, E. M., Marcovitch, S., Calkins, S. & Weaver, J. M. (2012). Developmental dynamics of emotion and cognition processes in preschoolers. Child Development, 84, 346 – 360. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Calkins, S. D. & Marcovitch, S. (2010). Emotion regulation and executive functioning in early development: Integrated mechanisms of control supporting adaptive functioning. In S. D. CalkinsM. A. BellEds., Child development at the intersection of emotion and cognition (pp. 59 – 78). Washington: APA. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cameron Ponitz, C. C., McClelland, M. M., Jewkes, A. M., McDonald Connor, C., Farris, C. L. & Morrison, F. J. (2008). Touch your Toes! Developing a direct measure of behavioral regulation in early childhood. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 23, 141 – 158. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cole, P. M., Tan, P. Z., Hall, S. E., Zhang, Y., Crnic, K. A., Blair, C. B. & Li, R. (2011). Developmental Changes in Anger Expression and Attention Focus: Learning to Wait. Developmental Psychology, 47, 1078 – 1089. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Denham, S. A., Caverly, S., Schmidt, M., Blair, K., DeMulder, E., Caal, S., Hamada, H. & Mason, T. (2002). Preschool understanding of emotions: Contributions to classroom anger and aggression. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 901 – 916. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Duncan, G. J., Dowsett, C. J., Claessens, A. M. K., Huston, A. C., Klebanov, P., Pagani, L. S. et al. (2007). School readiness and later achievement. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1428 – 1446. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Goodman, R. (1997). The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 581 – 586. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Grimm, H. (2001). SETK 3 – 5. Sprachentwicklungstest für drei- bis fünfjährige Kinder (2. Aufl.). Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Grimm, K. J., Steele, J. S., Mashburn, A., Burchinal, M. & Pianta, R. C. (2010). Early behavioral associations of achievement trajectories. Developmental Psychology, 46, 976 – 983. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gross, J. J. & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 348 – 362. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hawes, D. J. & Dadds, M. R. (2004). Australian data and psychometric properties of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 38, 644 – 651 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hölling, H., Erhardt, M., Ravens-Sieberer, U. & Schlack, R. (2007). Verhaltensauffälligkeiten bei Kindern und Jugendlichen. Erste Ergebnisse aus dem Kinder- und Jugendgesundheitssurvey (KiGGS). Bundesgesundheitsblatt–Gesundheitsforschung–Gesundheitsschutz, 50, 784 – 793. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Janke, B. (2008). Emotionswissen und soziale Kompetenz. Zeitschrift für empirische Pädagogik, 22, 127 – 144. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kats-Gold, I. & Priel, B. (2009). Emotion, understanding, and social skills among boys at risk of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychology in the Schools, 46, 658 – 678. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kim, J. & Deater-Deckard, K. (2011). Dynamic changes in anger, externalizing, and internalizing problems: Attention and regulation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 156 – 166. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Leonard, M., Milich, R., & Lorch, E. P. (2011). The role of pragmatic language use in mediating the role between hyperactivity and social skills problems. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 54, 567 – 579. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mostow, A., Izard, C. E., Fine, S. & Trentacosta, C. (2002). Modeling emotional, cognitive, and behavioral predictors of peer acceptance. Child Development, 73, 1775 – 1787. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Polderman, T. C., Gosso, M. F., Posthuma, D., van Beijsterveldt, T. E. C., Heutink, P., Verhulst, F. C. & Boomsma, D. I. (2006). A longitudinal twin study on IQ, executive functioning, and attention problems during childhood and early adolescence. Acta Neurologica Belgica, 106, 191 – 207. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Pons, F. & Harris, P. L. (2002). Test of Emotion Comprehension –Version 2. Oxford: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ricken, G. & Fritz, A. (2006). Arbeitsgedächtnisleistungen bei unterschiedlich guten Rechnern im Kindergartenalter. Psychologie in Erziehung und Unterricht, 58, 263 – 274. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Röthlisberger, M., Neuenschwander, R., Michel, E. & Roebers, C. M. (2010). Exekutive Funktionen: Zugrundeliegende kognitive Prozesse und deren Korrelate bei Kindern im späten Vorschulalter. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 42, 99 – 100. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Salisch, M. von (2000). Wenn Kinder sich ärgern. Emotionsregulierung in der Entwicklung. Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Salisch, M. von, Hänel, M. & Freund, P. A. (2013). Emotion understanding and cognitive abilities in young children. Learning and Individual Differences, 26, 15 – 19. doi./10.1016/j.lindif.2013.04.001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schlack, R., Hölling, H., Kurth, B.-M. & Huss, M. (2007). Die Prävalenz der Aufmerksamkeitsdefizit-/Hyperaktivitätsstörung (ADHS) bei Kindern und Jugendlichen in Deutschland. Bundesgesundheitsblatt–Gesundheitsforschung–Gesundheitsschutz, 50, 827 – 835. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schwenck, C., Schmitt, D., Sievers, S., Romanos, M., Warnke, A. & Schneider, W. (2011). Kognitive und emotionale Empathie bei Kindern und Jugendlichen mit ADHS und Störungen des Sozialverhaltens. Zeitschrift für Kinder und Jugendpsychiatrie, 39, 265 – 276. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Toplak, M. E., West, R. F. & Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Practitioner review: Do performance-based measures and ratings of executive function assess the same construct? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54, 131 – 143. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Trentacosta, C. J., Izard, C. E., Mostow, A. J. & Fine, S. E. (2006). Children’s emotional competence and attentional competence in early elementary school. School Psychology Quarterly, 21, 148 – 170. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Trentacosta, C. & Izard, C. (2007). Kindergarten children’s emotion competence as a predictor of their academic competence in first grade. Emotion, 7, 77 – 88. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., Nigg, J. T., Faraone, S. V. & Pennington, B. F. (2005). Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Biological Psychiatry, 57,1336 – 1346. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wolf, C. (1995). Sozio-Ökonomischer Status und berufliches Prestige. Zuma-Nachrichten 37, 19, 102 – 136. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar