Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1026/2191-9186/a000115

Die explorative Studie hatte zum Ziel, die Auswirkungen von Spielumwelten im Kindergarten auf Geschlechtertrennung im freien Spiel sowie auf Geschlechterunterschiede im Spielverhalten und der Entwicklung von Mädchen und Jungen in den Bereichen sozial-emotionale Kompetenzen und räumliches Denken zu untersuchen. Die geschlechtstypischen Spielbereiche Bau- und Puppenecke wurden zu einem geschlechtsneutral gestalteten Spielbereich zusammengelegt. Vor und zwei Monate nach der Umgestaltungsmaßnahme wurden in der Kindergartengruppe (14 Mädchen, 10 Jungen) das gleich- und gemischtgeschlechtliche Spiel und das Spielverhalten im Freispiel beobachtet sowie die sozial-emotionalen Kompetenzen und das räumliche Denken der Kinder erfasst. Nach der Umgestaltungsmaßnahme nahm das gemeinsame Spiel von Mädchen und Jungen signifikant zu und im Spielverhalten waren keine Geschlechterunterschiede mehr zu beobachten. Bei den Jungen stieg die Beschäftigung mit Fantasie- und Rollenspielen signifikant an und sie verbesserten sich in ihren sozial-emotionalen Kompetenzen. Die Mädchen spielten signifikant häufiger Konstruktionsspiele und verbesserten sich im räumlichen Denken.


Classroom environments: Effects on gender differences in play behavior and competence development

The primary goal of this explorative study was to investigate whether the classroom environment in kindergarten influences gender segregation and gender differences in play behavior and competence development. The gender-typed block and doll play areas were integrated to a gender-neutral designed play area. The same-sex and mixed-sex play and the play behavior were observed during free-play and the children's social-emotional competencies and spatial abilities were assessed before and two months after the change in the classroom design. After the intervention the common play of boys and girls increased, and no gender differences in play behavior were observed anymore. Boys were more engaged in pretend and sociodramatic play and improved their social-emotional competencies. Girls were more engaged in construction play and improved their spatial abilities.

Literatur

  • Ashiabi, G. S. (2007). Play in the preschool classroom: Its socioemotional significance and the teacher's role in play. Early Childhood Education Journal, 35 (2), 199 – 207. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Blank-Mathieu, M. (1996). Jungen im Kindergarten. Frankfurt am Main: Brandes und Apsel. Google Scholar

  • Bretherton, I. (1989). Pretense. The form and function of make-believe play. Developmental Review, 9, 383 – 401. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bronfenbrenner, U. (1990). Ökologische Sozialisationsforschung. In L. Kruse, C.-F. Graumann & E.-D. Lantermann (Hrsg.), Ökologische Psychologie. Ein Handbuch in Schlüsselbegriffen (S. 76 – 79). München: Psychologie Verlags Union. Google Scholar

  • Brosnan, M. J. (1998). Spatial ability in children's play with Lego blocks. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 87, 19 – 28. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Caldera, Y. M. , McDonald-Culp, A. , O'Brien, M. , Truglio, R. T. , Alvarez, M. , Huston, A. C. (1999). Children's play preferences, construction play with blocks and visualspatial skills. Are they related? International Journal of Behavioral Development, 23, 855 – 872. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Casey, B. , Andrews, N. , Schindler, H. , Kersh, J. E. , Samper, A. , Copley, J. (2008). The development of spatial skills through interventions involving block building activities. Cognition & Instruction, 26 (3), 269 – 309. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Casey, B. , Bobb, B. , Sarama, J. , Clements, D. (2003). The power of block building. Teaching Children Mathematics, 10 (2), 98 – 102. Google Scholar

  • Christie, J. F. , Johnsen, E. P. (1983). The role of play in social-intellectual development. Review of Educational Research, 53, 93 – 115. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Clark, A. H. , Wyon, S. M. , Richards, M. P. M. (1969). Free play in nursery school children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 10, 205 – 216. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Coates, S. , Lord, M. , Jakabovics, E. (1975). Field, dependence-independence, social-nonsocial play and sex differences in preschool children. Perceptual and motor skills, 40, 195 – 202. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Connolly, J. A. , Doyle, A.-B. (1984). Relation of social fantasy play to social competence in preschoolers. Developmental Psychology, 20, 797 – 806. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Connor, J. M. , Serbin, L. S. (1977). Behaviorly based masculine and feminine activity – reference scales for preschoolers: Correlates with other classroom behaviors and cognitive tests. Child Development, 48, 1411 – 1416. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Connor, J. M. , Serbin, L. S. , Schackman, M. (1977). Sex differences in children ' s responses to training on a visual-spatial test. Developmental Psychology, 13, 293 – 294. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Döpfner, M. , Berner, W. , Fleischmann, T. , Schmidt, M. (1993). Verhaltensbeurteilungsbogen für Vorschulkinder (VBV 3 – 6). Weinheim: Beltz. Google Scholar

  • Eagly, A. H. (1995). The science and politics of comparing women and men. American Psychologist, 50, 145 – 158. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Einsiedler, W. (1984). Die Bedeutung des Spiels für die kindliche Entwicklung im Vorschulalter. Erlangen-Nürnberg: IfG-Institut für Grundschulforschung. Google Scholar

  • Einsiedler, W. (1999). Das Spiel der Kinder. Zur Pädagogik und Psychologie des Kinderspiels. Bad Heilbrunn: Klinkhardt. Google Scholar

  • Eisenberg, N. , Fabes, R. A. , Bernzwieg, J. , Karbon, M. , Poulin, R. , Hanish, L. (1993). The relations of emotionality and regulation to preschooler ' s social skills and sociometric status. Child Development, 64, 1418 – 1438. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Eisenberg, N. , Tryon, K. , Cameron, E. (1984). The relations of preschoolers ' peer interaction to their sex-typed toy choices. Child Development, 55, 45 – 70. Google Scholar

  • Elias, C. L. , Berk, L. E. (2002). Self-regulation in young children. Is there a role for sociodramatic play? Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 17, 216 – 238. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Eliot, L. (2001). Was geht da drinnen vor? Die Gehirnentwicklung in den ersten fünf Lebensjahren. Berlin: Berlin Verlag. Google Scholar

  • Fabes, R. A. , Eisenberg, N. , Jones, S. , Smith, M. , Guthrie, I. , Poulin, R. , Shepard, S. , Friedman, J. (1999). Regulation, emotionality, and preschoolers ' socially competent peer interactions. Child Development, 70, 432 – 442. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fabes, R. A. , Martin, C. L. , Hanish, L. D. (2003). Young children ' s play qualities in same-, other-, and mixed-sex peer groups. Child Development, 74 (3), 921 – 932. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fantuzzo, J. , Sekino, Y. , Cohen, H. (2004). An examination of the contributions of interactive peer play to salient classroom competencies for urban Head Start children. Psychology in the Schools, 41, 323 – 336. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fein, G. G. (1981). Pretend play in childhood: An integrative review. Child Development, 52, 1095 – 1118 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fein, G. G. (1989). Mind, meaning, and affect: Proposals for a theory of pretense. Developmental Review, 9, 345 – 363. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fisher, E. P. (1992). The impact of play on development: A meta-analysis. Play & Culture, 5 (2), 159 – 181. Google Scholar

  • Hoffmann, J. , Russ, S. (2012). Pretend play, creativity, and emotion regulation in children. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity and the Arts, 6 (2), 175 – 184. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hughes, F. (2010). Children, Play, and Development. SAGE Publications. Google Scholar

  • Kavanaugh, R. D. (2006). Pretend play and theory of mind. In L. Balter & C. S. Tamis-LeMonda (Eds.), Child psychology: A handbook of contemporary issues (pp. 153 – 166). Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press. Google Scholar

  • Kinsman, C. A. , Berk, L. E. (1979). Joining the block and housekeeping areas. Changes in play and social behavior. Young Children, 35, 66 – 75. Google Scholar

  • Kersh, J. , Casey, B. , Young, J. M. (2008). Research on spatial skills and block building in girls and boys: The relationship to later mathematics learning. In O. N. Saracho & B. Spodek (Eds.), Contemporary perspectives on mathematics in early childhood education (pp. 233 – 252). Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing. Google Scholar

  • Krafft, K. C. , Berk, L. E. (1998). Private speech in two preschools: Significance of open-ended activities and make-believe play for verbal self-regulation. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 13, 637 – 658. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ladd, G. W. , Herald, S. L. , Andrews, R. K. (2006). Young children ' s peer relations and social competence. In. B. Spodek & O. N. Saracho (Eds.), Handbook of research on the education of young children (pp. 23 – 54). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Google Scholar

  • LaFreniere, P. J. , Dumas, J. E. (1996). Social competence and behavioral evaluation in children ages 3 to 6 years. The short form (SCBE-30). Psychological Assessment, 8, 369 – 377. Google Scholar

  • Lillard, A. (2002). Pretend play and cognitive development. In U. Goswami (Ed.), Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (pp. 188 – 205). London: Blackwell Publishing. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lindsey, E. W. , Colwell, M. J. (2003). Preschoolers ' emotional competence: Links to pretend and physical play. Child Study Journal, 33, 39 – 52. Google Scholar

  • Linn, M. C. , Petersen, A. C. (1985). Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability. A meta-analysis. Child Development, 56, 1479 – 1498. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lloyd, B. , Smith, C. (1985). The social representation of gender and young children ' s play. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 3, 65 – 73. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lohaus, A. , Schumann-Hengsteler, R. , Kessler, T. (1999). Räumliches Denken im Kindesalter. Göttingen: Hogrefe. Google Scholar

  • Maccoby, E. E. (1998). The two sexes: Growing up apart, coming together. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Google Scholar

  • Maccoby, E. E. (2000). Psychologie der Geschlechter. Sexuelle Identität in den verschiedenen Lebensphasen. Stuttgart: Klett-Cotta. Google Scholar

  • Maccoby, E. E. , Jacklin, C. N. (1987). Gender segregation in childhood. In H. W. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (pp. 239 – 287). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Manaster, H. , Jobe, M. (2012). Preschoolers' Positive Peer Relationships. Young Children, 13, 12 – 17. Google Scholar

  • Martin, C. L. , Fabes, R. A. (2001). The stability and consequences of young children's same-sex peer interactions. Developmental Psychology, 37 (3), 431 – 446. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mayr, T. , Ulich, M. (2003). Die Engagiertheit von Kindern. Zur systematischen Reflexion von Bildungsprozessen in Kindertageseinrichtungen. In W. E. Fthenakis (Hrsg.), Elementarpädagogik nach PISA. Wie aus Kindertagesstätten Bildungseinrichtungen werden können (S. 169 – 189). Freiburg im Breisgau: Herder. Google Scholar

  • Mayr, T. , Ulich, M. (2009). Social-emotional well-being and resilience of children in early childhood settings – PERIK: an empirically based observation scale for practitioners. Early Years – An International Journal of Research and Development, 29 (1), 45 – 58. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McAloney, K. , Stagnitti, K. (2009). Pretend play and social play: The concurrent validity of the Child-Initiated Pretend Play Assessment. International Journal of Play Therapy, 18, 99 –113. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mehta, C. M. , Strough, J. (2009). Sex segregation in friendships and normative contexts across the life span. Developmental Review, 29 (3), 201 – 220. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Moore, M. , Russ, S. (2008). Follow-up of a pretend play intervention: Effects on play, creativity, and emotional processes in children. Creativity Research Journal, 20, 427 – 436. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ness, D. , Farenga, S. J. (2007). Knowledge under construction: The importance of play in developing children's spatial and geometric thinking. Lanhan, MD: Rowman & Littlefield. Google Scholar

  • Nickel, H. , Schmidt-Denter, U. (1988). Vom Kleinkind zu Schulkind. Eine entwicklungspsychologische Einführung für Erzieher, Lehrer und Eltern. München: Ernst Reinhardt. Google Scholar

  • Oerter, R. (1993). Kinderspiel. In M. Markefka & B. Nauck (Hrsg.), Handbuch der Kindheitsforschung (S. 377 – 388). Neuwied: Luchterhand. Google Scholar

  • Oerter, R. (1998). Kindheit. In R. Oerter & L. Montada (Hrsg.), Entwicklungspsychologie. Ein Lehrbuch (S. 249 – 309). Weinheim: Beltz. Google Scholar

  • Ogawa, M. , Takahashi, N. (2012). The developmental relationship between role play, pretend play, and theory of mind in young children. Japanese Journal Of Developmental Psychology, 23 (1), 85 – 94. Google Scholar

  • Pellegrini, A. P. , Bjorklund, D. F. (2004). The ontogeny and phylogeny of children's object and fantasy play. Human Nature, 15 (1), 23 – 43. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pellegrini, A. D. , Perlmutter, J. C. (1989). Classroom contextual effects on children's play. Developmental Psychology, 25 (2), 289 – 296. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pepler, D. J. , Ross, H. S. (1981). The effects of play on convergent and divergent problem-solving. Child Development, 52, 1202 – 1210. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Permien, H. , Frank, K. (1995). Schöne Mädchen – Starke Jungen? Gleichberechtigung. (k)ein Thema in Tageseinrichtungen für Schulkinder. Freiburg: Lambertus. Google Scholar

  • Preiser, S. (1985). Die Beobachtung des Spielgeschehens. In W. Einsiedler (Hrsg.), Aspekte des Kinderspiels. Pädagogisch-psychologische Spielforschung (S. 237 – 248). Weinheim: Beltz. Google Scholar

  • Quaiser-Pohl, C. (2001). Raumvorstellungsfähigkeit. In G. Wenninger (Hrsg.), Lexikon der Psychologie. M-Ref (S. 422 – 423). Heidelberg: Spektrum. Google Scholar

  • Rogers, A. , Russo, S. (2003). Blocks: A commonly encountered play activity in the early years, or a key to facilitating skills in science, maths and technology? Investigating. Australian Primary & Junior Science Journal, 19 (1), 17 – 21. Google Scholar

  • Rohrmann, T. , Thoma, P. (1998). Jungen in Kindertagesstätten. Ein Handbuch zur geschlechtsbezogenen Pädagogik. Freiburg im Breisgau: Lambertus. Google Scholar

  • Ruble, D. N. , Martin, C. L. (1998). Gender development. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol.3. Social, emotional, and personality development (pp. 933 – 1016). New York: Wiley. Google Scholar

  • Salisch, M. von (2002). Emotionale Kompetenz entwickeln. Grundlagen in Kindheit und Jugend. Stuttgart: Kohlhammer. Google Scholar

  • Shea, D. L. , Lubinski, D. , Benbow, C. P. (2001). Importance of assessing spatial ability in intellectually talented young adolescents: A 20-year longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93 (3), 604 – 614. Google Scholar

  • Singer, J. L. (1998). Imaginative play in early childhood. A foundation for adaptive emotional and cognitive development. International Medical Journal, 5, 93 – 100. Google Scholar

  • Smith, P. K. , Dutton, S. (1979). Play and training in direct and innovative problem solving. Child Development, 50, 830 – 836. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Snijders, J. T. , Snijders-Oomen, N. (1970). Nicht-verbale Intelligenzuntersuchung für Hörende und Taube. Groningen: Walters-Noordhoff. Google Scholar

  • Sodian, B. , Thoermer, C. (2006). Theory of Mind. In W. Schneider & B. Sodian (Hrsg.), Kognitive Entwicklung (Enzyklopädie der Psychologie, Themenbereich C: Theorie und Forschung, Serie V: Entwicklungspsychologie, Bd. 2, S. 495 – 612). Göttingen: Hogrefe. Google Scholar

  • Stanat, P. , Kunter, M. (2001). Geschlechterunterschiede in Basiskompetenzen. In J. Baumert et al. (Hrsg.), PISA 2000. Basiskompetenzen von Schülerinnen und Schülern im internationalen Vergleich (S. 249 – 269). Opladen: Leske + Budrich. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stannard, L. , Wolfgang, C. H. , Jones, I. , Phelps, P. (2001). A longitudinal study of the predictive relations among construction play and mathematical achievement. Early Child Development and Care, 167, 115 – 125. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sylva, K. , Bruner, J. S. , Genova, P. (1976). The role of play in the problem-solving of children 3 – 5 years old. In J. S. Bruner, A. Jolly & K. Sylva (Eds.), Play: Its role in development and evolution (pp. 244 – 260). Harmondsworth: Penguin. Google Scholar

  • Theokas, C. , Ramsey, P. G. , Sweeney, B. (1993, March). The effects of classroom interventions on young children's cross-sex contacts and perceptions. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development. New Orleans, LA. Google Scholar

  • Tracy, D. M. (1987). Toys, spatial ability, and science and mathematics achievement: Are they related? Sex Roles, 17, 115 – 138. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tröster, H. , Reineke, D. (2007). Prävalenz von Verhaltens- und Entwicklungsauffälligkeiten im Kindergartenalter. Ergebnisse einer Erhebung in Kindergärten eines Landkreises. Kindheit und Entwicklung, 16, 171 – 179. LinkGoogle Scholar

  • Uren, N. , Stagnitti, K. (2009). Pretend play, social competence and involvement in children aged 5 – 7 years: The concurrent validity of the Child-Initiated Pretend Play Assessment. Australian Occupational Therapy Journal, 56, 33 – 40. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Google Scholar

  • Walden, R. , Schmitz, I. (1999). KinderRäume. Kindertagesstätten aus architekturpsychologischer Sicht. Freiburg im Breisgau: Lambertus. Google Scholar

  • Wellman, M. (1990). The child's theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books/MIT Press. Google Scholar

  • Wolfgang, C. H. , Stannard, L. , Jones, I. (2003). Advanced constructional play with Legos among preschoolers as a predictor of later school achievement in mathematics. Early Child Development & Care, 173 (5), 467 – 475. CrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Youngblade, L. M. , Dunn, J. (1995). Individual differences in young children's pretend play with mother and sibling: Links to relationships and understanding of other people's feelings and beliefs. Child Development, 66, 1472–1492. Google Scholar