Skip to main content
Articles

Gender Differences in Psychophysiological Responses to Disgust

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803.22.2.65

Several studies have revealed that women report stronger feelings of disgust than men (Gross & Levenson, 1995; Schienle, Schäfer, Stark, Walter, & Vaitl, 2005). However, the extent to which this gender difference also influences physiological disgust responses remains an open question. In Experiment 1, 54 female and 41 male participants were exposed to slides of different disgust-content. In Experiment 2, 47 women and 53 men watched two film clips showing hygiene-related or food-related disgust stimuli, respectively. Differences between males and females in reported and physiological disgust responses (heart rate, electrodermal activity, salivary cortisol, secretory immunoglobulin A) were tested by analysis of variance. Replicating previous studies, women reported stronger feelings of disgust than men across all disgust inductions. Additionally, in Study 1, women showed a higher increase in skin conductance level than men. In conclusion, gender moderates subjective responses to disgust, whereas physiological disgust responses are only marginally moderated by gender. Gender stereotypes as an explanation for the results are discussed.

References

  • Alexander, M.G. , Wood, W. (2000). Women, men, and positive emotions: A social role interpretation. In A.H. Fischer (Ed.), Gender and emotion: Social psychological perspectives (2nd ed., pp. 189–210). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Azevedo, T.M. , Volchan, E. , Imbiriba, L.A. , Rodrigues, E.C. , Oliveira, J.M. , Oliveira, L.F. et al. (2005). A freezing-like posture to pictures of mutilation. Psychophysiology, 42, 255–260. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brody, L.R. , Hall, J.A. (2000). Gender, emotion, and expression. In M. Lewis,, J.M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 338–349). New York: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Buske-Kirschbaum, A. , Geiben, A. , Wermke, C. , Pirke, K.M. , Hellhammer, D.H. (2001). Preliminary evidence for herpes labialis recurrence following experimentally induced disgust. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 70, 86–91. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chentsova-Dutton, Y.E. , Tsai, J.L. (2007). Gender differences in emotional response among European Americans and among Americans. Cognition and Emotion, 21, 162–181. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Christie, I.C. , Friedman, B.H. (2004). Autonomic specificity of discrete emotion and dimensions of affective space: A multivariate approach. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 51, 143–153. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Comer, R.J. (2001). Klinische Psychologie (2. Auflage) [Clinical psychology]. München, Germany: Spektrum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Davey, G.C.L. (1994). Disgust. In V.S. Ramachandran (Ed.), Encyclopedia of human behavior (pp. 135–141). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Davidson, R.J. , Ekman, P. , Saron, C.D. , Senulis, J.A. , Friesen, W.V. (1990). Approach-withdrawal and cerebral asymmetry: Emotional expression and brain physiology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 330–341. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Diener, E. , Sandvik, E , Larsen, R.J. (1985). Age and sex effects for emotional intensity. Developmental Psychology, 21, 542–546. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Drummond, P.D. , Hewson-Bower, B. (1997). Increased psychosocial stress and decreased mucosal immunity in children with recurrent upper respiratory tract infections. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 43, 271–278. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Druschel, B. , Sherman, M.F. (1999). Disgust sensitivity as a function of the Big Five and gender. Personality and Individual Differences, 26, 739–748. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ekman, P. (1971). Universals and cultural differences in facial expressions of emotion. In J. Cole (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (pp. 207–283). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ekman, P. (1992). Facial expression and emotion. American Psychologist, 48, 384–392. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ekman, P. , Friesen, W.V. , O‘Sullivan, M. (1988). Smiles when lying. Interpersonal Relations and Group Processes, 54, 414–420. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Gross, J.J. (1998). Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: Divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 224–237. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gross, J.J. , Levenson, R.W. (1993). Emotional suppression: Physiology, self-report, and expressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 970–986. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gross, J.J. , Levenson, R.W. (1995). Emotion elicitation using films. Cognition and Emotion, 9, 87–108. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Grossman, M. , Wood, W. (1993). Sex Differences in intensity of emotional experience: A social role interpretation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 1010–1022. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Haidt, J. , McCauley, C. , Rozin, P. (1994). Individual differences in sensitivity to disgust: A scale sampling seven domains of disgust elicitors. Personality and Individual Differences, 16, 701–713. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hennig, J. (1994). Die psychobiologische Bedeutung des sekretorischen Immunglobulin A im Speichel [The psychobiological meaning of secretory immunoglobulin A in saliva]. New York: Waxmann. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hennig, J. , Pössel, P. , Netter, P. (1996). Sensitivity to disgust as an indicator of neuroticism: A psychobiological approach. Personality and Individual Differences, 20, 589–596. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hucklebridge, F. , Lambert, S. , Clow, A. , Warburton, D.M. , Evans, P.D. , Sherwood, N. (2000). Modulation of secretory immunoglobulin A in saliva: Response to manipulation of mood. Biological Psychology, 53, 25–35. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Janke, W. , Debus, G. (1978). Die Eigenschaftswörterliste (EWL). Eine Mehrdimensionale Methode zur Beschreibung von Aspekten des Befindens [A multidimensional mood checklist]. Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Johnsen, B. , Thayer, J.F. , Hugdahl, K. (1995). Affective judgment of Ekman faces: A dimensional approach. Journal of Psychophysiology, 9, 193–202. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kirschbaum, C. , Hellhammer, D.H. (1994). Salivary cortisol in psychoneuroendocrine research: recent developments and applications. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 19, 313–333. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kring, A.M. , Gordon, A.H. (1998). Sex differences in emotion: Expression, experience, and physiology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 683–703. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lang, P.J. , Bradley, M.M. , Cuthbert, B. (1995). International affective picture system (IAPS). Gainesville, FL: The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Lang, P.J. , Greenwald, M.K. , Bradley, M.M. , Hamm, A.O. (1993). Looking at pictures: Affective, facial, visceral and behavioral responses. Psychophysiology, 30, 261–273. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Levenson, R.W. , Carstensen, L.L. , Friesen, W.V. , Ekman, P. (1991). Emotion, physiology, and expression in old age. Psychology and Aging, 6, 28–35. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Levenson, R.W. , Ekman, P. , Friesen, W.V. (1990). Voluntary facial action generates emotion-specific autonomic nervous system activity. Psychophysiology, 27, 363–384. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lovallo, W.R. , Thomas, T.L. (2000). Stress hormones in psychophysiological research: Emotional, behavioral, and cognitive implications. In J.T. Cacioppo, K.G. Tassinary, G.G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology (2nd ed., pp. 342–367). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Nordin, S. , Broman, D.A. , Garvill, M. , Nyroos, M. (2004). Gender differences in factors affecting rejection of food in healthy young Swedish adults. Appetite, 43, 295–301. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Olatunji, B.O. , Williams, N.L. , Lohr, J.M. , Sawchuk, C.N. (2005). The structure of disgust: Domain specificity in relation to contamination ideation and excessive washing. Behavior Research and Therapy, 43, 1069–1086. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Oppliger, P.A. , Zillmann, D. (1997). Disgust in humor: Its appeal to adolescents. International Journal of Humor Research, 10, 421–437. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Power, M. , Dalgleish, T. (1997). Cognition and emotion. From order to disorder. Hove, UK: Psychology Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Quigley, J.F. , Sherman, M.F. , Sherman, N.C. (1997). Personality disorder symptoms, gender, and age as predictors of adolescent disgust sensitivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 22, 661–667. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Robin, O. , Rousmans, S. , Dittmar, A. , Vernet-Maury, E. (2003). Gender influence on emotional responses to primary tastes. Physiology and Behavior, 78, 385–393. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rohrmann, S. , Schienle, A. , Hodapp, V. , Netter, P. (2004). Experimentelle Überprüfung des Fragebogens zur Erfassung der Ekelempfindlichkeit (FEE) [An experimental validation of the questionnaire for the assessment of disgust sensitivity]. Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie und Psychotherapie, 33, 91–100. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Rozin, P. , Fallon, A. (1987). A perspective on disgust. Psychological Review, 94, 23–41. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rozin, P. , Haidt, J. , McCauley, C.R. (1993). Disgust. In M. Lewis, J.M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 575–594). New York: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Rozin, P. , Haidt, J. , McCauley, C.R. (2000). Disgust. In M. Lewis, J.M. Haviland-Jones. (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 637–653). New York: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Rozin, P. , Lowery, L. , Ebert, R. (1994). Varieties of disgust faces and the structure of disgust. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 870–881. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schienle, A. , Schäfer, A. , Stark, R. , Walter, B. , Vaitl, D. (2005). Gender differences in the processing of disgust- and fear-inducing pictures: An fMRI study. Neuroreport, 16, 277–280. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schienle, A. , Stark, R. , Vaitl, D. (2001). Evaluative conditioning: A possible explanation for the acquisition of disgust responses? Learning and Motivation, 32, 65–83. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schienle, A. , Stark, R. , Walter, B. , Blecker, C. , Ott, U. , Kirsch, P. , Sammer, G. , Vaitl, D. (2002). The insula is not specifically involved in disgust processing: A fMRI study. Neuroreport, 13, 2023–2026. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schienle, A. , Walter, B. , Stark, R. , Vaitl (2002). Ein Fragebogen zur Erfassung der Ekelempfindlichkeit (FEE) [A questionnaire for the assessment of disgust sensitivity]. Zeitschrift für Psychologie und Psychotherapie, 31, 110–120. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Seidlitz, L. , Diener, E. (1998). Sex differences in the recall of affective experiences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 262–271. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stark, R. , Schienle, A. , Walter, B. , Kirsch, P. , Sammer, G. , Ott, U. , Blecker, C. , Vaitl, D. (2003). Hemodynamic responses to fear and disgust-inducing pictures: An fMRI study. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 50, 225–234. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stark, R. , Walter, B. , Schienle, A. , Vaitl, D. (2005). Psychophysiological correlates of disgust and disgust sensitivity. Journal of Psychophysiology, 19, 50–60. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Suarez, E. C , Saab, P.G. , Llabre, M.M. , Kuhn, C.M. , Zimmerman, E. (2004). Ethnicity, gender, and age effects on adrenoceptors and physiological responses to emotional stress. Psychophysiology, 41, 450–460. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Templer, D.I. , King, F.L. , Brooner, R.K. , Corgiat, M. (1984). Assessment of body elimination attitude. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 3, 754–759. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tucker, M. , Bond, N.W. (1997). The roles of gender, sex role, and disgust in fear of animals. Personality and Individual Differences, 22, 135–138. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vrana, S.R. (1994). Startle reflex response during sensory modality specific disgust, anger and neutral imagery. Journal of Psychophysiology, 30, 279–286. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Woody, S.R. , Teachman, B.A. (2000). Intersection of disgust and fear: Normative and pathological views. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 7, 291–311. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Yartz, A.R. , Hawk, L.W. (2002). Addressing the specificity of affective startle modulation: Fear versus disgust. Biological Psychology, 59, 55–68. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar