Skip to main content
Article

Emotional Distraction by Constant Finger Tapping

Pupil Dilation and Decreased Unpleasant Emotion and Task-Unrelated Thoughts

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000287

Abstract. This study examined the effects of emotional distraction in the context of recalling stressful interpersonal events in daily life. Previous studies have revealed that distraction decreases unpleasant emotions. In this study, we examined whether distraction tasks decrease unpleasant task-related thoughts in addition to unpleasant emotions. Furthermore, to investigate the implicit effects of emotional changes, we examined changes in pupil size in relation to unpleasant emotions and task-unrelated thoughts (TUT). The behavioral tasks were administered to 21 university students (Mage = 21.24 ± 2.93 years; range: 18–30 years). After excluding participants for poor data quality, 16 (21.31 ± 3.34 years; 18–30 years) were included for further pupil data analysis. As an emotion induction procedure, participants were asked to freely recall memories of stressful interpersonal events in their daily lives and were given a series of questions about their recalled memories, which were presented on the monitor. In the following distraction experiment, questions during the emotion induction procedure were represented as emotional stimulation; a distraction task (non-constant or constant finger tapping) or rest condition was then performed; subsequently, ratings were given for attentional state, thought types conceived during the tasks, and emotional state. Upon analysis, differences in the ratings for unpleasant emotion and TUT were non-significant between all conditions. Nevertheless, pupil dilation in the non-constant and constant conditions was associated with decreased unpleasant emotions (p < .05; p < .05 to .01). More importantly, pupil dilation was associated with decreased unpleasant TUT (p < .05) only in the constant condition. Although the observed effects were subtle, we found that constant finger tapping decreased unpleasant emotions and TUT. It is expected for a future emotion regulation study to further investigate the effects in the relationships among emotions, thoughts, and physiological states, which can help in coping with unpleasant emotions.

References

  • Arabaci, G., & Parris, B. A. (2018). Probe-caught spontaneous and deliberate mind wandering in relation to self-reported inattentive, hyperactive and impulsive traits in adults. Scientific Reports, 8(1), Article 4113. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-22390-x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Beatty, J., & Lucero-Wagoner, B. (2000). The pupillary system. In J. T. CacioppoL. G. TassinaryG. G. BerntsonEds., Handbook of psychophysiology (pp. 14–162). Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Binder, J. R., Frost, J. A., Hammeke, T. A., Bellgowan, P. S. F., Rao, S. M., & Cox, R. W. (1999). Conceptual processing during the conscious resting state: A functional MRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(1), 80–93. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892999563265 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bradley, M. M., Miccoli, L., Escrig, M. A., & Lang, P. J. (2008). The pupil as a measure of emotional arousal and autonomic activation. Psychophysiology, 45(4), 602–607. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00654.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bradshaw, J. (1967). Pupil size as a measure of arousal during information processing. Nature, 216(5114), 515–516. https://doi.org/10.1038/216515a0 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Breiter, H. C., Etcoff, N. L., Whalen, P. J., Kennedy, W. A., Rauch, S. L., Buckner, R. L., Strauss, M. M., Hyman, S. E., & Rosen, B. R. (1996). Response and habituation of the human amygdala during visual processing of facial expression. Neuron, 17(5), 875–887. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80219-6 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cheyne, J. A., Carriere, J. S. A., & Smilek, D. (2006). Absent-mindedness: Lapses of conscious awareness and everyday cognitive failures. Consciousness and Cognition, 15(3), 578–592. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2005.11.009 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Christoff, K., Gordon, A. M., Smallwood, J., Smith, R., & Schooler, J. W. (2009). Experience sampling during fMRI reveals default network and executive system contributions to mind wandering. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(21), 8719–8724. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0900234106 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Frijda, N. H. (1988). The laws of emotion. The American Psychologist, 43(5), 349–358. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066x.43.5.349 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Giambra, L. M. (1995). A laboratory method for investigating influences on switching attention to task-unrelated imagery and thought. Consciousness and Cognition, 4(1), 1–21. https://doi.org/10.1006/ccog.1995.1001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gross, J. J. (1998). Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: Divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(1), 224–237. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.224 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gusnard, D. A., & Raichle, M. E. (2001). Searching for a baseline: Functional imaging and the resting human brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(10), 685–694. https://doi.org/10.1038/35094500 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Henderson, R. R., Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (2014). Modulation of the initial light reflex during affective picture viewing. Psychophysiology, 51(9), 815–818. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12236 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hu, N., He, S., & Xu, B. (2012). Different efficiencies of attentional orienting in different wandering minds. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(1), 139–148. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2011.12.007 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kalisch, R., Wiech, K., Herrmann, K., & Dolan, R. J. (2006). Neural correlates of self-distraction from anxiety and a process model of cognitive emotion regulation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(8), 1266–1276. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2006.18.8.1266 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kanske, P., Heissler, J., Schonfelder, S., Bongers, A., & Wessa, M. (2011). How to regulate emotion? Neural networks for reappraisal and distraction. Cerebral Cortex, 21(6), 1379–1388. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhq216 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Karatekin, C., Marcus, D. J., & Couperus, J. W. (2007). Regulation of cognitive resources during sustained attention and working memory in 10-year-olds and adults. Psychophysiology, 44(1), 128–144. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00477.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kreibig, S. D. (2010). Autonomic nervous system activity in emotion: A review. Biological Psychology, 84(3), 394–421. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.03.010 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Laeng, B., Sirois, S., & Gredeback, G. (2012). Pupillometry: A Window to the preconscious? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(1), 18–27. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691611427305 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1998). International affective pictures system (IAPS): Digitized photographs, instruction manual and affective ratings (Tech. Rep. A-6). University of Florida, NIMH Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Larson, D. G., & Chastain, R. L. (1990). Self-concealment: Conceptualization, measurement, and health implications. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 9(4), 439–455. https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.1990.9.4.439 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Manly, T., Robertson, I. H., Galloway, M., & Hawkins, K. (1999). The absent mind: Further investigations of sustained attention to response. Neuropsychologia, 37(6), 661–670. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00127-4 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Marchetti, I., Koster, E. H., & De Raedt, R. (2012). Mindwandering heightens the accessibility of negative relative to positive thought. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(3), 1517–1525. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2012.05.013 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mason, M. F., Norton, M. I., Van Horn, J. D., Wegner, D. M., Grafton, S. T., & Macrae, C. N. (2007). Wandering minds: The default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science, 315(5810), 393–395. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1131295 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McKiernan, K. A., Kaufman, J. N., Kucera-Thompson, J., & Binder, J. R. (2003). A parametric manipulation of factors affecting task-induced deactivation in functional neuroimaging. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(3), 394–408. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892903321593117 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McRae, K., Hughes, B., Chopra, S., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Gross, J. J., & Ochsner, K. N. (2010). The neural bases of distraction and reappraisal. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(2), 248–262. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21243 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McRae, K., Misra, S., Prasad, A. K., Pereira, S. C., & Gross, J. J. (2012). Bottom-up and top-down emotion generation: implications for emotion regulation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7(3), 253–262. https://doi.org/10.1093/Scan/Nsq103 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McVay, J. C., & Kane, M. J. (2009). Conducting the train of thought: Working memory capacity, goal neglect, and mind wandering in an executive-control task. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition, 35(1), 196–204. https://doi.org/10.1037/A0014104 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1991). A prospective-study of depression and posttraumatic stress symptoms after a natural disaster: The 1989 Loma-Prieta earthquake. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(1), 115–121. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.61.1.115 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1993). Effects of rumination and distraction on naturally-occurring depressed mood. Cognition & Emotion, 7(6), 561–570. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699939308409206 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ochsner, K. N., Ray, R. R., Hughes, B., McRae, K., Cooper, J. C., Weber, J., Gabrieli, J. D. E., & Gross, J. J. (2009). Bottom-up and top-down processes in emotion generation: Common and distinct neural mechanisms. Psychological Science, 20(11), 1322–1331. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02459.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Otto, B., Misra, S., Prasad, A., & McRae, K. (2014). Functional overlap of top-down emotion regulation and generation: An fMRI study identifying common neural substrates between cognitive reappraisal and cognitively generated emotions. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 14(3), 923–938. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-013-0240-0 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ozawa, S. (2021). Emotions induced by recalling memories about interpersonal stress. Frontiers in Psychology, 12(1157). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.618676 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ozawa, S., & Hiraki, K. (2017). Distraction decreases prefrontal oxygenation: A NIRS study. Brain and Cognition, 113, 155–163. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2017.02.003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ozawa, S., Yoshimoto, H., Okanoya, K., & Hiraki, K. (2020). Pupil constrictions and their associations with increased negative affect during responses to recalled memories of interpersonal stress. Journal of Psychophysiology, 35(3), 186–195. https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000273 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Partala, T., & Surakka, V. (2003). Pupil size variation as an indication of affective processing. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 59, 185–198. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1071-5819(03)00017-X First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pennebaker, J. W., & Oheeron, R. C. (1984). Confiding in others and illness rate among spouses of suicide and accidental-death victims. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93(4), 473–476. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843x.93.4.473 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Phelps, E. A., & LeDoux, J. E. (2005). Contributions of the amygdala to emotion processing: From animal models to human behavior. Neuron, 48(2), 175–187. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2005.09.025 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Robertson, I. H., Manly, T., Andrade, J., Baddeley, B. T., & Yiend, J. (1997). “Oops!”: Performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects. Neuropsychologia, 35(6), 747–758. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00015-8 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sato, A., & Yasuda, A. (2001). Development of the Japanese version of Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) scales. The Japanese Journal of Personality, 9, 138–139. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schönfelder, S., Kanske, P., Heissler, J., & Wessa, M. (2014). Time course of emotion-related responding during distraction and reappraisal. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(9), 1310–1319. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nst116 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Seli, P., Carriere, J. S. A., & Smilek, D. (2015). Not all mind wandering is created equal: dissociating deliberate from spontaneous mind wandering. Psychological Research, 79(5), 750–758. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-014-0617-x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Seli, P., Risko, E. F., Smilek, D., & Schacter, D. L. (2016). Mind-wandering with and without intention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(8), 605–617. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.05.010 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Shaw, G. A., & Giambra, L. M. (1992). Task-unrelated-thoughts of college-students diagnosed as hyperactive in childhood. International Journal of Psychology, 27(3–4), 390. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Shulman, G. L., Fiez, J. A., Corbetta, M., Buckner, R. L., Miezin, F. M., Raichle, M. E., & Petersen, S. E. (1997). Common blood flow changes across visual tasks: II. Decreases in cerebral cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9(5), 648–663. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1997.9.5.648 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Smallwood, J., Fitzgerald, A., Miles, L. K., & Phillips, L. H. (2009). Shifting moods, wandering minds: Negative moods lead the mind to wander. Emotion, 9(2), 271–276. https://doi.org/10.1037/A0014855 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2006). The restless mind. Psychological Bulletin, 132(6), 946–958. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.132.6.946 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stawarczyk, D., Majerus, S., Maj, M., Van der Linden, M., & D’Argembeau, A. (2011). Mind-wandering: Phenomenology and function as assessed with a novel experience sampling method. Acta Psychologica, 136(3), 370–381. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.01.002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Steinhauer, S. R., Siegle, G. J., Condray, R., & Pless, M. (2004). Sympathetic and parasympathetic innervation of pupillary dilation during sustained processing. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 52(1), 77–86. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2003.12.005 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stone, A. A., & Neale, J. M. (1984). New measure of daily coping: Development and preliminary results. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46(4), 892–906. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.46.4.892 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Thiruchselvam, R., Hajcak, G., & Gross, J. J. (2012). Looking inward: Shifting attention within working memory representations alters emotional responses. Psychological Science, 23(12), 1461–1466. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612449838 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Thompson, R. F. (2009). Habituation: A history. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 92(2), 127–134. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2008.07.011 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Van Dillen, L. F., Heslenfeld, D. J., & Koole, S. L. (2009). Tuning down the emotional brain: An fMRI study of the effects of cognitive load on the processing of affective images. NeuroImage, 45(4), 1212–1219. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.016 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063–1070. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wolgast, M., & Lundh, L.-G. (2017). Is distraction an adaptive or maladaptive strategy for emotion regulation? A person-oriented approach. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 39(1), 117–127. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-016-9570-x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar