Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759.24.4.210

This review discusses basic features of Implicit Association Tests (IATs) that are relevant in order to estimate their suitability of IATs for the valid assessment of individual differences. We start with a description of the essential characteristics of the procedures of IATs and also refer to theoretical accounts for IAT effects. Then, we provide an overview of research on the psychometric properties of IATs including their reliability and incremental validity for the prediction of behavior over and above explicit measures. Finally, we describe some alternatives to IAT measures and offer an outlook for future research.

References

  • Amodio, D.M. , Devine, P.G. (2006). Stereotyping and evaluation in implicit race bias: Evidence for independent constructs and unique effects on behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 652–661. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Asendorpf, J.B. , Banse, R. , Mücke, D. (2002). Double dissociation between implicit and explicit personality self-concept: The case of shy behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 380–393. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Back, M.D. , Schmukle, S.C. , Egloff, B. (2005). Measuring task-switching ability in the Implicit Association Test. Experimental Psychology, 52, 167–179. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Banse, R. (1999). Automatic evaluation of self and significant others: Affective priming in close relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 16, 803–821. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Banse, R. , Greenwald, A.G. (2007). Personality and implicit social cognition research: Past, present, and future. European Journal of Personality, 21, 371–382. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Blanton, H. , Jaccard, J. , Gonzales, P.M. , Christie, C. (2006). Decoding the Implicit Association Test: Implications for criterion prediction. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 192–212. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Boldero, J.M. , Rawlings, D. , Haslam, N. (2007) Convergence between GNAT-assessed implicit and explicit personality. European Journal of Personality, 21, 341–358. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bosson, J.K. , Swann, W.B. , Pennebaker, J.W. (2000). Stalking the perfect measure of implicit self-esteem: The blind men and the elephant revisited? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 631–643. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Conrey, F.R. , Sherman, J.W. , Gawronski, B. , Hugenberg, K. , Groom, C.J. (2005). Separating multiple processes in implicit social cognition: The quad model of implicit task performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 469–487. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • De Houwer, J. (2003a). A structural analysis of indirect measures of attitudes. In J. Musch, K.C. Klauer, (Ed.), The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion (pp. 219–244). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • De Houwer, J. (2003b). The extrinsic affective Simon task. Experimental Psychology, 50, 77–85. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • De Houwer, J. , Beckers, T. , Moors, A. (2007). Novel attitudes can be faked on the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 972–978. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • De Houwer, J. , De Bruycker, E. (2007). The Implicit Association Test outperforms the extrinsic affective Simon task as an implicit measure of interindividual differences in attitudes. British Journal of Social Psychology, 46, 401–421. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • De Houwer, J. , Geldof, T. , De Bruycker, E. (2005). The Implicit Association Test as a general measure of similarity. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 228–239. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Egloff, B. , Schmukle, S.C. (2002). Predictive validity of an Implicit Association Test for assessing anxiety. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1441–1455. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Egloff, B. , Schwerdtfeger, A. , Schmukle, S.C. (2005). Temporal stability of the Implicit Association Test-Anxiety. Journal of Personality Assessment, 84, 82–88. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ellwart, T. , Becker, E.S. , Rinck, M. (2005). Activation and measurement of threat associations in fear of spiders: An application of the Extrinsic Affective Simon Task. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 36, 281–299. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fazio, R.H. , Olson, M.A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition research: Their meaning and use. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 297–327. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fazio, R.H. , Sanbonmatsu, D.M. , Powell, M.C. , Kardes, F.R. (1986). On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 229–238. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fiedler, K. , Bluemke, M. (2005). Faking the IAT: Aided and unaided response control on the Implicit Association Test. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 27, 307–316. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Friese, M. , Hofmann, W. , Wänke, M. (in press). When impulses take over: Moderated predictive validity of explicit and implicit attitude measures in predicting food choice and consumption behavior. British Journal of Social Psychology. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A.G. (2005). The multifactor trait IAT. Unpublished manuscript. University of Washington, Seattle. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A.G. , Banaji, M.R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102, 4–27. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A.G. , McGhee, D.E. , Schwartz, J.L.K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A.G. , Nosek, B.A. , Banaji, M.R. (2003). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 197–216. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A.G. , Poehlman, T.A. , Uhlmann, E. , Banaji, M.R. (in press). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hofmann, W. , Gawronski, B. , Gschwendner, T. , Le, H. , Schmitt, M. (2005). A meta-analysis on the correlation between the Implicit Association Test and explicit self-report measures. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 1369–1385. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hofmann, W. , Gschwendner, T. , Nosek, B.A. , Schmitt, M. (2005). What moderates implicit-explicit consistency? European Review of Social Psychology, 16, 335–390. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hofmann, W. , Rauch, W. , Gawronski, B. (2007). And deplete us not into temptation: Automatic attitudes, dietary restraint, and self-regulatory resources as determinants of eating behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 479–504. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Huijding, J. , de Jong, P.J. (2005). A pictorial version of the Extrinsic Affective Simon Task. Experimental Psychology, 52, 289–295. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Karpinski, A. , Steinman, R.B. (2006). The single category Implicit Association Test as a measure of implicit social cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 16–32. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kim, D.Y. (2003). Voluntary controllability of the Implicit Association Test (IAT). Social Psychology Quarterly, 66, 83–96. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Klauer, K.C. , Voss, A. , Schmitz, F. , Teige-Mocigemba, S. (2007). Process components of the Implicit Association Test: A diffusion-model analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 353–368. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lane, K.A. , Banaji, M.R. , Nosek, B.A. , Greenwald, A.G. (2007). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: IV. What we know (so far). In B. Wittenbrink, N. Schwarz, (Eds.), Implicit measures of attitudes: Procedures and controversies (pp. 59–102). New York: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • McConnell, A.R. , Liebold, J.M. (2001). Relations among the Implicit Association Test, discriminatory behavior, and explicit measures of racial attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37, 435–442. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mierke, J. , Klauer, K.C. (2001). Implicit association measurement with the IAT: Evidence for effects of executive control processes. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 48, 107–122. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Mierke, J. , Klauer, K.C. (2003). Method-specific variance in the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 1180–1192. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nosek, B.A. (2005). Moderators of the relationship between implicit and explicit evaluation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134, 565–584. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nosek, B.A. , Banaji, M.R. (2001). The go/no-go association task. Social Cognition, 19, 625–666. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nosek, B.A. , Greenwald, A.G. , Banaji, M.R. (2006). The Implicit Association Test at age 7: A methodological and conceptual review. In J.A. Bargh, (Ed.), Social psychology and the unconscious: The automaticity of higher mental processes (pp. 265–292). New York: Psychology Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Nosek, B.A. , Smyth, F.L. , Hansen, J.J. , Devos, T. , Lindner, N.M. , Ranganath, K.A. et al. (2007). Pervasiveness and correlates of implicit attitudes and stereotypes. European Review of Social Psychology, 18, 36–88. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nosek, B.A. , Sriram, N. (2007). Faulty assumptions: A comment on Blanton, Jaccard, Gonzales, and Christie (2006). Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 393–398. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Olson, J.M. , Goffin, R.D. , Haynes, G.A. (2007). Relative versus absolute measures of explicit attitudes: Implications for predicting diverse attitude-relevant criteria. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 907–926. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Payne, B.K. , Cheng, C.M. , Govorun, O. , Stewart, B.D. (2005). An inkblot for attitudes: Affect misattribution as implicit measurement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 277–293. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Penke, L. , Eichstaedt, J. , Asendorpf, J.B. (2006). Single attribute Implicit Association Tests (SA-IAT) for the assessment of unipolar constructs: The case of sociosexuality. Experimental Psychology, 53, 283–291. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Perkins, A.W. , Forehand, M.R. (2006). Decomposing the implicit self-concept: The relative influence of semantic meaning and valence on attribute self-association. Social Cognition, 24, 387–408. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Perugini, M. (2005). Predictive models of implicit and explicit attitudes. British Journal of Social Psychology, 44, 29–45. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Perugini, M. , Prestwich, A. (2007). The gatekeeper: Individual differences are key in the chain from perception to behavior. European Journal of Personality, 21, 303–317. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schnabel, K. , Asendorpf, J.B. , Greenwald, A.G. (2008). Using Implicit Association Tests for the assessment of implicit personality self-concept. In G.J. Boyle, G. Matthews, and D.H. Saklofske, (Eds.), Handbook of personality theory and testing (pp. 508–528). London: Sage. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schnabel, K. , Banse, R. , Asendorpf, J.B. (2006a). Assessment of implicit personality self-concept using the Implicit Association Test (IAT): Concurrent assessment of anxiousness and angriness. British Journal of Social Psychology, 45, 373–396. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schnabel, K. , Banse, R. , Asendorpf, J.B. (2006b). Employing automatic approach and avoidance tendencies for the assessment of implicit personality self-concept: The Implicit Association Procedure (IAP). Experimental Psychology, 53, 69–76. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Schröder-Abé, M. , Rudolph, A. , Wiesner, A. , Schütz, A. (2007a). High implicit self-esteem is not necessarily advantageous: Discrepancies between explicit and implicit self-esteem and their relationship with anger expression and psychological health. European Journal of Personality, 21, 319–339. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schröder-Abé, M. , Rudolph, A. , Wiesner, A. , Schütz, A. (2007b). Self-esteem discrepancies and defensive reactions to social feedback. International Journal of Psychology, 42, 174–183. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sriram, N. , Greenwald, A.G. (2007). The Brief Implicit Association Test. Manuscript submitted for publication. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Steffens, M.C. (2004). Is the Implicit Association Test immune to faking. Experimental Psychology, 51, 165–179. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Steffens, M.C. , Kirschbaum, M. , Glados, P. (in press). Avoiding stimulus confounds in Implicit Association Tests by using the concepts as stimuli. British Journal of Social Psychology. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Steffens, M.C. , Plewe, I. (2001). Items’ cross-category associations as a confounding factor in the Implicit Association Test. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 48, 123–134. First citation in articleAbstractGoogle Scholar

  • Strack, F. , Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 220–247. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Teige-Mocigemba, S. , Klauer, K.C. , Rothermund, K. (2008). Minimizing method-specific variance in the IAT: A single block IAT. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 25, 237–245. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Teige, S. , Schnabel, K. , Banse, R. , Asendorpf, J.B. (2004). Assessment of multiple implicit self-concept dimensions using the extrinsic affective Simon task (EAST). European Journal of Personality, 18, 495–520. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Thush, C. , Wiers, R.W. , Ames, S.L. , Grenard, J.L. , Sussman, S. , Stacy, A.W. (in press). Interactions between implicit and explicit cognition and working memory capacity in the prediction of alcohol use in at-risk adolescents. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar