Skip to main content
Original Articles and Reviews

Strategies to Improve Name Learning

A Review

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040/a000363

Abstract. The following points emerge from the present review of strategies to improve the learning of proper names: (a) Face-name mnemonic techniques based on mental imagery have been shown to be efficient in laboratory settings in both young and older adults. Unfortunately, they are particularly effortful and require capacity for imagination, making them difficult to apply in a real conversational context. (b) Strategies based on spaced retrieval practice have been found to be efficient both in laboratory and more ecological settings, and both in young and older adults. (c) Techniques based on spaced retrieval practice appear to be more efficient than those based on mental imagery. (d) More recent research has proposed new perspectives, such as basing learning strategies on implicit, rather than explicit, memory processes such as hyper-binding. Finally, neuroscience research has started to investigate the possibility of using non-invasive electrical brain stimulation to improve name learning.

References

  • Allerton, D. J. (1996). Proper names and definite descriptions with the same reference: A pragmatic choice for language users. Journal of Pragmatics, 25, 621–633. https://doi.org/10.1016/0378-2166(94)00103-0 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Andrewes, D. G., Kinsella, G., & Murphy, M. (1996). Using a memory handbook to improve everyday memory in community-dwelling older adults with memory complaints. Experimental Aging Research, 22, 305–322. https://doi.org/10.1080/03610739608254013 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Barresi, A., Obler, L. K., & Goodglass, H. (1998). Dissociation between proper name and common noun learning. Brain and Cognition, 37, 21–23. https://doi.org/10.1006/brcg.1997.0987 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Biss, R. K., Rowe, G., Weeks, J. C., Hasher, L., & Murphy, K. J. (2018). Leveraging older adults’ susceptibility to distraction to improve memory for face-name associations. Psychology and Aging, 33, 158–164. https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000192 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bjork, R. A. (1975). Retrieval as a memory modifier: An interpretation of negative recency and related phenomena. In R. L. SolsoEd., Information processing and cognition: The Loyola Symposium (pp. 123–144). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Blumenfeld, R. S., Parks, C. M., Yonelinas, A. P., & Ranganath, C. (2011). Putting the pieces together: The role of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in relational memory encoding. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 257–265. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2010.21459 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brédart, S. (2017). The cognitive psychology and neuroscience of naming people. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 83, 145–154. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.10.008 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brooks, J. O., Friedman, L., Gibson, J. M., & Yesavage, J. A. (1993). Spontaneous mnemonic strategies used by older and younger adults to remember proper names. Memory, 1, 393–407. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658219308258245 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Campbell, K. L., Hasher, L., & Thomas, R. C. (2010). Hyper-binding: A unique age effect. Psychological Science, 21, 399–405. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797609359910 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carney, R. N., & Levin, J. R. (2012). Facing facts: Can the face-name mnemonic strategy accommodate additional factual information. Learning, Instruction, and Cognition, 80, 386–404. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220973.2011.630431 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Carney, R. N., & Levin, J. R. (2014). Learning more about with the face-name mnemonic strategy. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28, 569–578. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3036 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carney, R. N., Levin, J. R., & Stackhouse, T. L. (1997). The face-name mnemonic strategy from a different perspective. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 22, 399–412. https://doi.org/10.1006/ceps.1997.0940 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carpenter, S. K., & DeLosh, E. L. (2005). Applications of the testing and spacing effects to name learning. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19, 619–636. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1101 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carpenter, S. K., & DeLosh, E. L. (2006). Impoverished cue support enhances subsequent retention: Support for the elaborative retrieval explanation of the testing effect. Memory & Cognition, 34, 268–276. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193405 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cohen, G., Hummert, M. L., Wiemann, J. M., & Nussbaum, J. F. (1994). Age-related problems in the use of proper names in communication. In M. L. HummertJ. M. WiemannJ. F. Nussbaum (Eds.), Interpersonal communication in older adulthood: Interdisciplinary theory and research (pp. 40–57). London, UK: Sage. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781483326832.n3 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Crook, T. H., & West, R. L. (1990). Name recall performance across the adult life-span. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 335–349. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1990.tb02365.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cull, W. L., Shaughnessy, J. J., & Zechmeister, E. B. (1996). Expanding understanding of the expanding pattern-of-retrieval mnemonic: Toward confidence in applicability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 2, 365–378. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-898X.2.4.365 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Enfield, N. J., & Stivers, T. (2007). Person reference in interaction: linguistic, cultural and social perspectives. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fogler, K. A., James, L. E., & Crandall, E. A. (2010). How name descriptiveness impacts proper name learning in young and older adults. Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 17, 505–518. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825580903477245 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gardiner, J. M., Craik, F. I. M., & Bleasdale, F. A. (1973). Retrieval difficulty and subsequent recall. Memory & Cognition, 1, 213–216. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03198098 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gratzinger, O., Sheikh, J. I., Friedman, L., & Yesavage, J. (1990). Cognitive interventions to improve face-name recall: The role of personality traits differences. Developmental Psychology, 26, 889–893. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.26.6.889 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Griffin, Z. M. (2010). Retrieving personal names, referring expressions, and terms of address. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 53, 345–387. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(10)53009-3 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hanley, J. R. (2014). Retrieval failures for names of familiar people. In B. L. SchwartzA. S. BrownEds., Tip-of-the-tongue states and related phenomena (pp. 50–74). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139547383.004 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hastings, M. W. (1982). Effectiveness of face-name strategies. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 54, 167–170. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1982.54.1.167 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Helder, E., & Shaughnessy, J. J. (2008). Retrieval opportunities while multitasking improve name recall. Memory, 16, 896–909. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210802360611 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • James, L. E. (2004). Meeting Mister Farmer versus meeting a farmer: Specific effects of aging on learning proper names. Psychology and Aging, 19, 515–522. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.19.3.515 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • James, L. E., Tauber, S. K., McMahan, E. A., Oberle, S., Martinez, A. P., & Fogler, K. A. (2012). Searching for interference effects in learning new face-name associations. Memory, 20, 155–166. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2011.649290 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Landauer, T. K., & Bjork, R. A. (1978). Optimum rehearsal patterns and name learning. In M. M. GrunebergP. E. MorrisR. N. SykesEds., Practical aspects of memory (pp. 625–632). London, UK: Academic Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Leshikar, E. D., Leach, R. C., McCurdy, M. P., Trumbo, M. C., Sklenar, A. M., Frankenstein, A. N., & Matzen, L. E. (2017). Transcranial direct current stimulation of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during encoding improves recall but not recognition memory. Neuropsychologia, 106, 390–397. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.10.022 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lorayne, H. (1958). How to develop a super-power memory. Preston, NJ: Thomas. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Maddox, G. B., & Balota, D. A. (2012). Self control of when and how much to test face-name pairs in a novel spaced retrieval paradigm: An examination of age-related differences. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 19, 620–643. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2011.640658 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Matzen, L. E., Trumbo, M. C., Leach, R. C., & Leshikar, E. D. (2015). Effects of non-invasive brain stimulation on associative memory. Brain Research, 1624, 286–296. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2015.07.036 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McCarty, D. L. (1980). Investigation of a visual imagery mnemonic device for acquiring face-name associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 145–155. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.6.2.145 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McDermott, K. B., Arnold, K. M., & Nelson, S. M. (2014). The testing effect. In Perfect, T. J.Lindsay, D. S. (Eds.). The Sage handbook of applied memory (pp. 183–200). London, UK: Sage. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781446294703.n11 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Morris, C. D., Bransford, J. D., & Franks, J. J. (1977). Levels of processing versus transfer-appropriate processing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, 519–533. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(77)80016-9 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Morris, P. E., & Fritz, C. O. (2000). The name game: Using retrieval practice to improve the learning of names. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 6, 124–129. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-898X.6.2.124 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Morris, P. E., & Fritz, C. O. (2002). The improved name game: Better use of expanding retrieval practice. Memory, 10, 259–266. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210143000371 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Morris, P. E., Fritz, C. O., & Buck, S. (2004). The name game: Acceptability, bonus information and group size. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, 89–104. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.948 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Morris, P. E., Fritz, C. O., Jackson, L., Nichol, E., & Roberts, E. (2005). Strategies for learning proper names: Expanding retrieval practice, meaning and imagery. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19, 779–798. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1115 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Morris, P. E., Jones, S., & Hampson, P. (1978). An imagery mnemonic for the learning of names. British Journal of Psychology, 69, 335–336. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1978.tb01663.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Neuschatz, J. S., Preston, E. L., Toglia, M. P., & Neuschatz, J. S. (2005). Comparison of the efficacy of two name-learning techniques: Expanding rehearsal and face-name imagery. American Journal of Psychology, 118, 79–102. Retrieved from https://www.jstor.org/stable/30039044 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Nyström, S. (2016). Names and naming. In C. HoughEd., The Oxford handbook of names and naming (pp. 39–51). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199656431.013.26 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Old, S. R., & Naveh-Benjamin, M. (2012). Age differences in memory for names: The effect of prelearned semantic associations. Psychology and Aging, 27, 462–473. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025194 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Patton, G. W. R. (1994). Testing the efficacy of name mnemonics used during conversation. Psychological Reports, 75, 131–142. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1994.75.1.131 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pisoni, A., Vernice, M., Iasevoli, L., Cattaneo, Z., & Papagno, C. (2015). Guess who? Investigating the proper name processing networks by means of tDCS. Neuropsychologia, 66, 267–278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.11.025 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Roediger, H. L., & Karpicke, J. D. (2006). The power of testing memory: Basic research and implications for educational practice. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 249–255. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00012.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Semenza, C. (2011). Naming with proper names: The left temporal pole theory. Behavioural Neurology, 24, 277–284. https://doi.org/10.3233/BEN-2011-0338 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sheikh, J. I., Hill, R. D., & Yesavage, J. A. (1986). Long-term efficacy of cognitive training for Age-Associated Memory Impairment: A six month follow-up study. Developmental Neuropsychology, 2, 413–421. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565648609540358 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Weeks, J. C., Biss, R. K., Murphy, K. L., & Hasher, L. (2016). Face-name learning in older adults: A benefit of hyper-binding. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 23, 1559–1565. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-016-1003-z First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Weinstein, Y., McDermott, K. B., & Szpunar, K. K. (2011). Testing protects against proactive interference in face-name learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 518–523. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-011-0085-x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Yesavage, J. A., & Rose, T. L. (1984). The effects of a face-name mnemonic in young, middle-aged and elderly adults. Experimental Aging Research, 10, 55–57. https://doi.org/10.1080/03610738408258543 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Yesavage, J. A., Rose, T. L., & Bower, G. H. (1983). Interactive imagery and affective judgments improve face-name learning in the elderly. Journal of Gerontology, 38, 197–203. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronj/38.2.197 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Yesavage, J. A., Sheikh, J. I., Friedman, L., & Tanke, E. (1990). Learning mnemonics: Roles of aging and subtle cognitive impairment. Psychology and Aging, 5, 133–137. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.5.1.133 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar