Skip to main content
Original Article

The Right Hemisphere and Ambiguity

A Possible Role of Handedness in Rorschach Responsivity

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1192-5604/a000152

Abstract. The Rorschach Test has evolved from an idiosyncratic projective personality test to an evidence-based performance test used widely in forensic settings. The current paper argues that Rorschach researchers should consider the role of two related neuropsychological factors in the assessment of Rorschach responding: the role of the right cerebral hemisphere in the perception of ambiguous figures and the role of individual differences (as a function of consistency of handedness) in responding to ambiguous stimuli. The right hemisphere is more fluent and flexible in the perception of ambiguous stimuli. Moreover, individuals with mixed/inconsistent hand preference have greater access to right hemisphere processing, and, accordingly, are more fluent and flexible in their perception of ambiguous stimuli. This raises the possibility of quantitative and qualitative differences in Rorschach responsivity as a function of test takers’ handedness. Implications of the presence of higher rates of schizotypy in inconsistent-handers are also discussed.

References

  • Asari, T., Konishi, S., Jimura, K., Chikazoe, J., Nakamura, N., & Miyashita, Y. (2008). Right temporopolar activation associated with unique perception. NeuroImage, 41(1), 145–152. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.01.059 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Barone, M. B., Lyle, K. B., & Winterich, K. P. (2015). When deal depth doesn’t matter: How handedness consistency influences consumer response to horizontal versus vertical price comparisons. Marketing Letters, 26(2), 213–223. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11002-013-9276-8 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Belyi, B. I. (1983). Reflection of functional hemispheric asymmetry in some phenomena of visual perception. Human Physiology, 8, 410–417. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Birch, H. G., & Belmont, I. (1961). Functional levels of disturbance manifested by brain-damaged (left hemiplegic) patients as revealed in Rorschach responses. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 132(5), 410–416. https://doi.org/10.1097/00005053-196105000-00005 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brugger, P., & Regard, M. (1995). Rorschach inkblots in the peripheral visual fields: Enhanced associative quality to the left of fixation. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 156(3), 385–387. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221325.1995.9914831 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brugger, P., Regard, M., Landis, T., Cook, N., Krebs, D., & Niederberger, J. (1993). “Meaningful” patterns in visual noise: Effects of lateral stimulation and the observer’s belief in ESP. Psychopathology, 26(5–6), 261–265. https://doi.org/10.1159/000284831 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bryson, F. M., Grimshaw, G. M., & Wilson, M. S. (2009). The role of intellectual openness in the relationship between hand preference and positive schizotypy. Laterality, 14(5), 441–456. https://doi.org/10.1080/13576500802349684 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Burin, D., Pignolo, C., Ales, F., Giromini, L., Pyasik, M., Ghirardello, D., Zennaro, A., Angiletta, M., Castellino, L., & Pia, L. (2019). Relationships between personality features and the rubber hand illusion: An exploratory study. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, Article 2762. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02762 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chan, E. Y. (2018). Handedness and religious beliefs: Testing the two possible accounts of authoritarianism and belief updating. Personality and Individual Differences, 127, 101–106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.02.005 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Christman, S. D. (2013). Handedness and “earedness”: Strong right-handers are less likely to prefer obscure musical genres. Psychology of Music, 41(1), 89–96. https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735611415751 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Christman, S. D. (2014). Individual differences in personality as a function of degree of handedness: Consistent-handers are less sensation seeking, more authoritarian, and more sensitive to disgust. Laterality, 17, 354–367. https://doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2013.838962 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Christman, S. D., Bentle, M., & Niebauer, C. L. (2007). Handedness differences in body image distortion and eating disorder symptomatology. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 40, 247–256. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.20357 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Christman, S. D., Henning, B. R., Geers, A. L., Propper, R. E., & Niebauer, C. L. (2008). Mixed-handed persons are more easily persuaded and are more gullible: Interhemispheric interaction and belief updating. Laterality, 13(5), 403–426. https://doi.org/10.1080/13576500802079646 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Christman, S. D., Jasper, J. D., Sontam, V., & Cooil, B. (2007). Individual differences in risk perception versus risk taking: Handedness and interhemispheric interaction. Brain and Cognition, 63(1), 51–58. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2006.08.001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Christman, S. D., & Prichard, E. C. (2016). Half oaks, half willows: Degree, not direction, of handedness underlies both stable prevalence in the human population and species-beneficial variations in cognitive flexibility. Evolutionary Psychological Science, 2, 228–236. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40806-016-0047-7 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Christman, S., & Propper, R. (2010). An interhemispheric basis for episodic memory: Effects of handedness and bilateral eye movements. In G. DaviesD. WrightEds., Current issues in applied memory (pp. 185–205). Psychology Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Christman, S. D., Sontam, V., & Jasper, J. D. (2009). Individual differences in ambiguous figure perception: Degree of handedness and interhemispheric interaction. perception, 38, 1183–1198. https://doi.org/10.1068/p6131 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Davidson, R. J. (1993). Cerebral asymmetry and emotion: Conceptual and methodological conundrums. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 115–138. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699939308409180 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dragovic, M., & Hammond, G. (2005). Handedness and schizophrenia: A quantitative review of evidence. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 111, 410–419. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.2005.00519.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Exner, J. E. Jr. (1994). Rorschach and the study of the individual. Rorschachiana, 19(1), 7–23. https://doi.org/10.1027/1192-5604.19.1.7 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Finn, J. A., & Neuringer, C. (1968). Left-handedness: A study of its relation to opposition. Journal of Projective Techniques & Personality Assessment, 32, 49–52. https://doi.org/10.1080/0091651X.1968.10120446 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gainotti, G., de Rosa, E., & Fischetti, C. (1984). Spécialisation hémisphérique et traitement analytique et gestaltique des informations. Une recherche avec le test de Rorschach [Hemispheric specialization and analytical and gestaltic processing of information. Research with the Rorschach Test]. Revue de Psychologie Appliquée, 34, 89–98. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Gill, H. S., & O’Boyle, M. (2003). Generating an image from an ambiguous visual input: An electroencephalographic (EEG) investigation. Brain and Cognition, 51, 287–293. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00032-0 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Giromini, L., Viglione, D. J., Pineda, J., Porcelli, P., Hubbard, D., Zennaro, A., & Cauda, F. (2019). Human movement responses to the Rorschach and mirroring activity: An fMRI study. Assessment, 26, 56–69. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191117731813 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Giromini, L., Viglione, D. J., Zennara, A., & Cauda, F. (2016). Neural activity during production of Rorschach responses: An fMRI study. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 262, 25–31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2017.02.001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hall, M. M., Hall, G. C., & Lavoie, P. (1968). Ideation in patients with unilateral or bilateral midline brain lesions. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 73, 526–531. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0026515 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Henninger, P. (1996). Inkblot testing of commisurotomy subjects: Contrasting modes of organizing reality. In S. HameroffA. W. KaszniakA. C. ScottEds., Toward a science of consciousness: The first Tucson discussions and debates (pp. 203–221). MIT Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hirotoshi, H., Haida, M., Matsumoto, M., Hayakawa, N., Inomata, S., & Matsumoto, H. (2012). Differences of prefrontal cortex activity between picture-based personality tests: A near-infrared spectroscopy study. Journal of Personality Assessment, 94, 366–371. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2012.666597 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ishibashi, M., Uchiumi, C., Jung, M., Aizawa, N., Makita, K., Nakamura, Y., & Saito, D. N. (2016). Difference in brain hemodynamics in response to achromatic and chromatic cards of the Rorschach: A fMRI study. Rorschachiana, 37(1), 41–57. https://doi.org/10.1027/1192-5604/a000076 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Jasper, J. D., Kunzler, J., Prichard, E. C., & Christman, S. D. (2014). Individual differences in information order effects: The importance of right-hemisphere access in belief updating. Acta Psychologica, 148, 115–122. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.01.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kircher, T. T. J., Liddle, P. F., Brammer, M. J., Williams, S. C. R., Murray, R. M., & McGuire, P. K. (2002). Reversed lateralization of temporal activation during speech production in thought disordered patients with schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 32, 439–449. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291702005287 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kumar, S., Saini, R., & Jain, R. (2020). Hand preference and intolerance of uncertainty: Atypical cerebral lateralization advantages lower intolerance of uncertainty. Laterality, 25(1), 22–42. https://doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2019.1611843 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lanning, M. D., Christman, S. D., & Fugget, A. (in press). Social comparison and handedness: Mixed handers are less susceptible to egocentric biases in judgments about others’ performance. Personality and Individual Differences. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Lewis, R. T. (1979). Organic signs, creativity, and personality characteristics of patients following cerebral commissurotomy. Clinical Neuropsychology, 1, 29–33. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Lyle, K. B., & Grillo, M. C. (2014). Consistent-handed individuals are more authoritarian. Laterality, 19(2), 146–163. https://doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2013.783044 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Luders, E., Cherbuin, N., Thompson, P. M., Gutman, B., Anstey, K. J., Sachdev, P., & Toga, A. W. (2010). When more is less: Associations between corpus callosum size and handedness lateralization. NeuroImage, 52, 43–49. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.016 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lyle, K. B., & Grillo, M. C. (2020). Why are consistently-handed individuals more authoritarian? The role of need for cognitive closure. Laterality, 25(4), 490–510. https://doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2020.1765791 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Maurer, A. (1990). Preference and classification of emotional stimuli: A lateralized tachistoscopic experiment (Diploma thesis). Seminar of Applied Psychology. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Mihura, J. L., & Meyer, G. J. (2018). Introduction to R-PAS. In J. L. MihuraG. J. MeyerEds., Using the Rorschach Performance Assessment System (R-PAS) (pp. 3–22). Guilford Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Minor, S. W., White, H., & Owings, E. P. (1989). Hemispheric asymmetries in interpreting forms vs colors in ambiguous patterns. Brain and Cognition, 9, 123–135. https://doi.org/10.1016/0278-2626(89)90048-1 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Niebauer, C., Aselage, J., & Schutte, C. (2002). Interhemispheric interaction and consciousness: Degree of handedness predicts the intensity of a sensory illusion. Laterality, 7(1), 85–96. https://doi.org/10.1080/13576500143000159 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Niebauer, C. L., & Garvey, K. (2004). Gödel, Escher, and degree of handedness: Differences in interhemispheric interaction predict differences in understanding self-reference. Laterality, 9(1), 19–34. https://doi.org/10.1080/13576500342000130 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–113. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Petot, J.-M. (2005). Are the relationships between NEO PI-R and Rorschach markers of openness to experience dependent on the patient’s test-taking attitude? Rorschachiana, 27(1), 30–50. https://doi.org/10.1027/1192-5604.27.1.30 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Prichard, E., Propper, R. E., & Christman, S. D. (2013). Degree of handedness, but not direction, is a systematic predictor of cognitive performance. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 3–6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00009 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Propper, R. E., Pierce, J., Geisler, M. W., Christman, S. D., & Bellorado, N. (2012). Asymmetry in resting alpha activity: Effects of handedness. Open Journal of Medical Psychology, 1, 86–90. https://doi.org/10.4236/ojmp.2012.14014 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Raymond, M., & Pontier, D. (2004). Is there geographical variation in human handedness? Laterality, 9(1), 35–52. https://doi.org/10.1080/13576500244000274 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Regard, M. (1991). The perception and control of emotion: Hemispheric differences and the role of the frontal lobes (Habilitation thesis). University of Zürich. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Regard, M., & Landis, T. (1997). Hemispheric differences in the processing of ambiguity: Tachistoscopic studies with inkblots. Rorschachiana, 22(1), 114–129. https://doi.org/10.1027/1192-5604.22.1.114 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Regard, M., Landis, T., & Bash, K. W. (1985). Affectivity: Tachistoscopic evidence of hemispheric differences. In F. SchmielauEd., Psychologie in der Medizin (pp. 10–11). München. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Rose, J. P., Jasper, J. D., & Corser, R. (2012). Interhemispheric interaction and egocentrism: The role of handedness in social comparative judgement. British Journal of Social Psychology, 51, 111–129. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.2010.02007.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sergent, J. (1982). The cerebral balance of power: Confrontation or cooperation? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8, 253–272. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.8.2.253 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sergent, J., & Binik, Y. M. (1979). On the use of symmetry in the Rorschach Test. Journal of Personality Assessment, 43, 355–359. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4304_3 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Shobe, E. R., Ross, N. M., & Fleck, J. I. (2009). Influence of handedness and bilateral eye movements on creativity. Brain and Cognition, 71(3), 204–214. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2009.08.017 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Somers, M., Sommer, I. E., Boks, M. P., & Kahn, R. S. (2009). Hand-preference and population schizotypy: A meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Research, 108, 25–32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2008.11.010 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sommer, I., Ramsey, N., Kahn, S., Aleman, A., & Bouma, A. (2001). Handedness, language lateralisation and anatomical asymmetry in schizophrenia: Meta-analysis. British Journal of Psychiatry, 178, 344–351. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.178.4.344 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sontam, V., & Christman, S. D. (2012). Semantic organization and handedness: Mixed-handedness is associated with more diffuse activation of ambiguous word associates. Laterality, 17(1), 38–50. https://doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2010.529450 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sontam, V., Christman, S. D., & Jasper, J. D. (2009). Individual differences in semantic switching flexibility: Effects of handedness. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15, 1023–1027. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617709990440 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tran, U. S., Stieger, S., & Voracek, M. (2015). Mixed-footedness is a more relevant predictor of schizotypy than mixed-handedness. Psychiatry Research, 225, 446–451. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2014.11.069 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Van Kleeck, M. (1989). Hemispheric differences in global versus local processing of hierarchical visual stimuli by normal subjects: New data and a meta-analysis of previous studies. Neuropsychologia, 27, 1165–1178. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(89)90099-7 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vilkki, J. (1987). Ideation and memory in the inkblot technique after focal cerebral lesions. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 9, 699–710. https://doi.org/10.1080/01688638708405211 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar