Skip to main content
Original Article

Interactive Effects of Personality and Emotional Suppression on Sympathetic Activation

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1614-0001/a000114

The present research examined the moderating effects of four personality traits (Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, and Neuroticism) and their facets as measured by NEO-PI-R (Costa & McCrae, 2005) on the relationship between emotion suppression and sympathetic activation. Peripheral pulse amplitude (PPA), spontaneous skin conductance response fluctuations (SCR), and skin conductance level (SCL) were recorded in 129 university students, who were given instruction to behave as usual or to suppress their emotions while watching neutral and emotional movie clips. The results show that only agreeableness and one of its facets, tendermindedness (A6), moderated the effects of suppression on changes in PPA and SCR. While participants were watching emotional movie clip under the instruction to suppress, the level of sympathetic arousal in high agreeable persons increased more compared to those with lower agreeableness. These results were explained by the mechanisms related to the arousability and trait-incongruence responses.

References

  • Bjorklund, D. F. , Kipp, K. (1996). Parental investment theory and sex differences in the evolution of inhibition mechanisms. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 163–188. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bono, J. E. , Vey, M. A. (2007). Personality and emotional performance: Extraversion, neuroticism and self-monitoring. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 12, 177–192. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Boucsein, W. (1992). Electrodermal activity. New York: Plenum. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bradley, M. M. (2004). Emotion and motivation. In J. T. Cacioppo L. G. Tassinary G. G. BerntsonEds., Handbook of psychophysiology (pp. 602–642). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Butler, E. A. , Egloff, B. , Wilhelm, F. H. , Smith, N. C. , Erickson, E. A. , Gross, J. J. (2003). The social consequences of expressive suppression. Emotion, 3, 48–67. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Costa, Jr., P. T. , McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor (NEO-FFI) professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Costa, Jr., P. T. , McCrae, R. R. (2005). NEO PI-R Revidirani NEO inventar ličnosti. Jastrebarsko: Naklada Slap. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Dawson, M. E. , Schell, A. M. , Filion, D. L. (2004). The electrodermal system. In J. T. Cacioppo L. G. Tassinary G. G. BerntsonEds., Handbook of psychophysiology (pp. 200–223). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Eysenck, H. J. (1990). Biological dimensions of personality. In L. A. Pervin, (Ed.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 244–276). New York: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Gross, J. J. (1998). Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: Divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 224–237. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gross, J. J. , John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 348–362. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gross, J. J. , Levenson, R. W. (1993). Emotional suppression: Physiology, self-report, and expressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 970–986. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gross, J. J. , Levenson, R. W. (1995). Emotion elicitation using films. Cognition and Emotion, 9, 87–108. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gross, J. J. , Levenson, R. W. (1997). Hiding feelings: The acute effects of inhibiting negative and positive emotion. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 95–103. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gross, J. J. , Richards, J. M. , John, O. P. (2006). Emotion regulation in everyday life. In D. K. Snyder J. A. Simpson J. N. HughesEds., Emotion regulation in families: Pathways to dysfunction and health (pp. 13–35). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Iani, C. , Gopher, D. , Lavie, P. (2004). Effects of task difficulty and invested mental effort on peripheral vasoconstriction. Psychophysiology, 41, 789–798. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • John, O. P. , Srivastava, S. (1999). The Big Five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and theoretical perspectives. In L. A. Pervin O. P. JohnEds., Handbook of personality (pp. 102–138). New York: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kaye, T. (1998). American history X. Zagreb: UCD. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kunzmann, U. , Kupperbusch, C. S. , Levenson, R. W. (2005). Behavioral inhibition and amplification during emotional arousal: A comparison of two age groups. Psychology and Aging, 20, 144–158. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Robinson, J. L. , Demaree, H. A. (2009). Experiencing and regulating sadness: Physiological and cognitive effects. Brain and Cognition, 70, 13–20. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rohrmann, S. , Hennig, J. , Netter, P. (1999). Changing psychobiological stress reactions by manipulating cognitive processes. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 33, 149–161. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rohrmann, S. , Hennig, J. , Netter, P. (2002). Manipulation of physiological and emotional responses to stress in repressors and sensitizers. Psychology and Health, 17, 583–596. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rothbart, M. K. , Sheese, B. E. (2007). Temperament and emotion regulation. In J. Gross, (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 331–350). New York: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Schaefer, A. , Nils, F. , Sanchez, X. , Philippot, P. (2010). Assessing the effectiveness of a large database of emotion-eliciting films: A new tool for emotion researchers. Cognition and Emotion, 24, 1153–1172. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Smith, T. W. , Houston, B. K. , Stucky, R. J. (1985). Effects of threat of shock and control over shock on finger pulse volume, pulse rate and systolic blood pressure. Biological Psychology, 20, 31–38. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tobin, R. M. , Graziano, W. G. (2006). Development of regulatory processes through adolescence: A review of empirical studies. In D. Mroczek T. LittleEds., Handbook of personality development (pp. 263–283). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Tobin, R. M. , Graziano, W. G. , Vanman, E. J. , Tassinary, L. G. (2000). Personality, emotional experience, and efforts to control emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 656–669. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Watson, D. , Clark, L. A. (1992). On traits and temperament: General and specific factors of emotional experience and their relation to five-factor model. Journal of Personality, 60, 442–476. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar