Skip to main content
Original Article

Is It Only the Violence?

The Effects of Violent Video Game Content, Difficulty, and Competition on Aggressive Behavior

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-1105/a000291

Abstract. The results of prior research investigating whether the violence in violent video games leads to increased subsequent aggression are mixed. Some observers question whether the difficulty and/or the competitive aspects of these games are important, but overlooked, factors that also affect aggression. In the present study, participants (N = 408) played a violent or nonviolent video game that was either difficult or easy and in which they competed and won, competed and lost, or did not compete against another player. Results revealed that participants became more aggressive only after playing a competitive, as opposed to a noncompetitive, game. Level of violence, winning or losing, and game difficulty did not have any significant effect. These results support the assertion that competition in video games has an independent and significant effect on subsequent aggression beyond violent content and game difficulty.

References

  • Adachi, P. C., & Willoughby, T. (2011). The effect of videogame competition and violence on aggressive behavior: Which characteristic has the greatest influence? Psychology of Violence, 1(4), 259–274. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024908 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Adachi, P. C., & Willoughby, T. (2013). Demolishing the competition: The longitudinal link between competitive video games, competitive gambling, and aggression. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 42(7), 1090–1104. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-013-9952-2 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Anderson, C. A., & Dill, K. E. (2000). Video games and aggressive thoughts, feelings, and behavior in the laboratory and in life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 772–790. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.78.4.772 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Anderson, C. A., & Morrow, M. (1995). Competitive aggression without interaction: Effects of competitive versus cooperative instructions on aggressive behavior in video games. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 1020–1030. https://doi.org/10.1177/01461672952110003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Anderson, C. A., Shibuya, A., Ihori, N., Swing, E. L., Bushman, B. J., Sakamoto, A., Rothstein, H.R., & Saleem, M. (2010). Violent video game effects on aggression, empathy, and prosocial behavior in Eastern and Western countries: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 136(2), 151–173. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018251 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Archer, J. (2004). Sex differences in aggression in real-world settings: A meta-analytic review. Review of General Psychology, 8(4), 291–322. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.8.4.291 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Archer, J. (2006). Testosterone and human aggression: An evaluation of the challenge hypothesis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 319–345. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.12.007 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bartholow, B. D., & Anderson, C. A. (2002). Effects of violent video games on aggressive behavior: Potential sex differences. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38(3), 283–290. https://doi.org/10.1006/jesp.2001.1502 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Berkowitz, L. (1989). Frustration-aggression hypothesis: Examination and reformulation. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 59–73. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.106.1.59 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Breuer, J., Scharkow, M., & Quandt, T. (2015). Sore losers? A reexamination of the frustration–aggression hypothesis for colocated video game play. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 4(2), 126–137. https://doi.org/10.1037/ppm0000020 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bryant, F. B., & Smith, B. D. (2001). Refining the architecture of aggression: A measurement model for the Buss–Perry Aggression Questionnaire. Journal of Research in Personality, 35(2), 138–167. https://doi.org/10.1006/jrpe.2000.2302 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Busching, R., Gentile, D. A., Krahé, B., Möller, I., Khoo, A., Walsh, D. A., & Anderson, C. A. (2015). Testing the reliability and validity of different measures of violent video game use in the United States, Singapore, and Germany. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 4(2), 97–111. https://doi.org/10.1037/ppm0000004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Calvert, S. L., Appelbaum, M., Dodge, K. A., Graham, S., Nagayama Hall, G. C., Hamby, S., Fasig-Caldwell, L. G., Citkowicz, M., Galloway, D. P., & Hedges, L. V. (2017). The American Psychological Association Task Force assessment of violent video games: Science in the service of public interest. American Psychologist, 72(2), 126–143. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0040413 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Deutsch, M. (1993). Educating for a peaceful world. American Psychologist, 48(5), 510–517. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.48.5.510 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • de Vrieze, J. (2018). Meta-analyses were supposed to end scientific debates. Often, they only cause more controversy. Science, https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aav4617 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Dransfield, I. (2014). Doom 2: Hell on Earth turns 20 years old this week, and it’s still great. https://www.digitalspy.com/videogames/retro-gaming/a603734/doom-2-hell-on-earth-turns-20-years-old-this-week-and-its-still-great/ First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Elson, M., Breuer, J., Van Looy, J., Kneer, J., & Quandt, T. (2015). Comparing apples and oranges? Evidence for pace of action as a confound in research on digital games and aggression. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 4(2), 112–125. https://doi.org/10.1037/ppm0000010 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ferguson, C. J. (2015). Do angry birds make for angry children? A meta-analysis of video game influences on children’s and adolescents’ aggression, mental health, prosocial behavior and academic performance. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(5), 646–666. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615592234 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ferguson, C. J., Copenhaver, A., & Markey, P. (2020). Reexamining the findings of the American Psychological Association’s 2015 Task Force on Violent Media: A meta-analysis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620927666 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gershgorn, D. (2018). The game that kicked off a video game revolution turns 25 today. https://qz.com/1490069/doom-the-game-that-kicked-off-a-video-game-revolution-turns-25-today/ First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Greitemeyer, T., & Osswald, S. (2010). Effects of prosocial video games on prosocial behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98(2), 211–221. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016997 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Griffiths, R. P., Eastin, M. S., & Cicchirillo, V. (2016). Competitive video game play: An investigation of identification and competition. Communication Research, 43(4), 468–486. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650214565895 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hilgard, J. (2014). Hilgard’s modified video game paradigm. https://osf.io/3cb9m First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hilgard, J., Engelhardt, C. R., & Rouder, J. N. (2017). Overstated evidence for short-term effects of violent games on affect and behavior: A reanalysis of Anderson et al. (2010). Psychological Bulletin, 143(7), 757–774. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000074 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hilgard, J., Engelhardt, C. R., Rouder, J. N., Segert, I. L., & Bartholow, B. D. (2019). Null effects of game violence, game difficulty, and 2d:4d digit ratio on aggressive behavior. Psychological Science, 30(4), 606–616. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797619829688 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • id Software. (1994). Doom II: Hell on earth. [Video game]. GT Interactive Software. https://www.digitalspy.com/videogames/retro-gaming/a603734/doom-2-hell-on-earth-turns-20-years-old-this-week-and-its-still-great/ First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Lapierre, M. A., & Farrar, K. M. (2018). Learning to love guns? Gun-based gameplay’s links to gun attitudes. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 7(3), 216–230. https://doi.org/10.1037/ppm0000132 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mathur, M. B., & VanderWeele, T. J. (2019). Finding common ground in meta-analysis ”wars” on violent video games. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14(4), 705–708. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691619850104 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McCarthy, R. J., Coley, S. L., Wagner, M. F., Zengel, B., & Basham, A. (2016). Does playing video games with violent content temporarily increase aggressive inclinations? A pre-registered experimental study. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 67, 13–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2015.10.009 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McCarthy, R. J., & Elson, M. (2018). A conceptual review of lab-based aggression paradigms. Collabra: Psychology, 4(1), 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.104 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McGloin, R., Farrar, K. M., Krcmar, M., Park, S., & Fishlock, J. (2016). Modeling outcomes of violent video game play: Applying mental models and model matching to explain the relationship between user differences, game characteristics, enjoyment, and aggressive intentions. Computers in Human Behavior, 62, 442–451. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.04.018 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mohseni, M. R., Liebold, B., & Pietschmann, D. (2015). Extensive modding for experimental game research. In P. LankoskiS. BjörkEds., Game research methods (pp. 323–340). ETC Press. https://dl.acm.org/doi/abs/10.5555/2812774.2812799 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Nelson, R. J., & Trainor, B. C. (2007). Neural mechanisms of aggression. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 536–546. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Prescott, A. T., Sargent, J. D., & Hull, J. G. (2018). Meta-analysis of the relationship between violent video game play and physical aggression over time. PNAS Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 115(40), 9882–9888. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1611617114 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Saleem, M., Anderson, C. A., & Barlett, C. P. (2015). Assessing helping and hurting behaviors through the Tangram help/hurt task. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41, 1345–1362. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167215594348 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Saleem, M., Anderson, C. A., & Gentile, D. A. (2012). Effects of prosocial, neutral, and violent video games on children’s helpful and hurtful behaviors. Aggressive Behavior, 38(4), 281–287. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21428 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schmider, E., Ziegler, M., Danay, E., Beyer, L., & Bühner, M. (2010). Is it really robust? Reinvestigating the robustness of ANOVA against violations of the normal distribution assumption. Methodology: European Journal of Research Methods for the Behavioral and Social Sciences, 6(4), 147–151. https://doi.org/10.1027/1614-2241/a000016 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Schmierbach, M. (2010). “Killing spree”: Exploring the connection between competitive gameplay and aggressive cognition. Communication Research, 37(2), 256–274. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650209356394 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Shafer, D. M. (2012). Causes of state hostility and enjoyment in player versus player and player versus environment video games. Journal of Communication, 62(4), 719–737. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2012.01654.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Shao, R., & Wang, Y. (2019). The relation of violent video games to adolescent aggression: An examination of moderated mediation effect. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, Article 384. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00384 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Snyder, M. (1974). Self-monitoring of expressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30(4), 526–537. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0037039 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stevens, J. P. (2009). Applied multivariate statistics for the social sciences (5th ed.). Routledge\Taylor & Francis Group. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Von Neumann, J., & Morgenstern, O. (1944). Theory of games and economic behavior, Princeton University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Zimprich, D., Perren, S., & Hornung, R. (2005). A two-level confirmatory factor analysis of a modified Rosenberg self-esteem scale. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 65(3), 465–481. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164404272487 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar