Skip to main content
Original Article

The Effects of Counterfactual Attacks on Social Judgments

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000195

Two experiments were conducted to compare the effects of different styles of verbal criticism (factual vs. counterfactual) on the perceptions of target, source, and quality of the attack. Counterfactual attacks resulted in more negative overall judgment of the target and ratings of the target’s morality than either factual attacks or no attack. Counterfactual attacks were also rated more positively than factual attacks, and the source of the counterfactual attack was rated as being less biased against the target. Regression analyses confirmed that the observed effect on overall judgment was mediated by the perceived bias of the source. The greater effectiveness of counterfactual attacks was moderated by awareness of prior hostility of the source of the attack toward the target.

References

  • Abele, A. E. , Bruckmüller, S. (2011). The bigger one of the “Big Two”? Preferential processing of communal information. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 935–948. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Abele, A. E. , Uchronski, M. , Suitner, C. , & Wojciszke, B. (2008). Towards an operationalization of the fundamental dimensions of agency and communion: Trait content ratings in five countries considering valence and frequency of word occurrence. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 1202–1217. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Alicke, M. D. , Buckingham, J. , Zell, E. , Davis, T. (2008). Culpable control and counterfactual reasoning in the psychology of blame. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 1371–1381. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Barisione, M. , Catellani, P. (2008). L’offerta personalizzata degli sfidanti. ITANES. (Ed.), Il ritorno di Berlusconi. Vincitori e vinti nelle elezioni del 2008 [The return of Berlusconi: Winners and losers in 2008 elections.]. (pp. 137–148). Bologna, Italy : Il Mulino. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Baron, R. M. , Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bavelas, J. B. , Black, A. , Bryson, L. , Mullett, J. (1988). Political equivocation: A situational explanation. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 7, 137–145. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bertolotti, M. , Catellani, P. , Douglas, K. M. , & Sutton, R. M. (2013). The “Big Two” in political communication: The effects of attacking and defending politicians’ leadership or morality. Social Psychology, 44, 117–128. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Birch, S. , & Allen, N. (2010). How honest do politicians need to be? The Political Quarterly, 81, 49–56. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brambilla, M. , Rusconi, P. , Sacchi, S. , Cherubini, P. (2011). Looking for honesty: The primary role of morality (vs. sociability and competence) in information gathering. European Journal of Social Psychology, 41, 135–143. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brown, P. , & Levinson, S. C. (1987). Politeness: Some universals in language usages. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Budesheim, T. L. , Houston, D. A. , DePaola, S. J. (1996). Persuasiveness of in-group and out-group political messages: The case of negative political campaigning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 523–534. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bull, P. E. , Fetzer, A. (2010). Face, facework and political discourse. International Review of Social Psychology, 23, 155–185. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bull, P. E. , Wells, P. (2012). Adversarial discourse in Prime Minister’s Questions. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 31, 30–48. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cacioppo, J. T. , Gardner, W. L. , Berntson, G. G. (1997). Beyond bipolar conceptualizations and measures: The case of attitudes and evaluative space. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 1, 3–25. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carraro, L. , Gawronski, B. , Castelli, L. (2010). Losing on all fronts: The effects of negative versus positive person-based campaigns on implicit and explicit evaluations of political candidates. British Journal of Social Psychology, 49, 453–470. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Catellani, P. , Alberici, A. I. , Milesi, P. (2004). Counterfactual thinking and stereotypes: The nonconformity effect. European Journal of Social Psychology, 34, 421–436. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Catellani, P. , Bertolotti, M. (2014). The effects of counterfactual defences on social judgements. European Journal of Social Psychology. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.1995. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Catellani, P. , Covelli, V. (2013). The strategic use of counterfactual communication in politics. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 32, 495–504. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Catellani, P. , Bertolotti, M. , Covelli, V. (2013). Counterfactual communication in politics: Features and effects on voters. In I. Poggi, F. D’Errico, L. Vincze, E. A. Vinciarelli, (Eds.), Multimodal communication in political speech (pp. 75–85). Springer Verlag: Berlin. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Catellani, P. , Milesi, P. (2001). Counterfactuals and roles: Mock victims’ and perpetrators’ accounts of judicial cases. European Journal of Social Psychology, 31, 247–264. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Catellani, P. , Milesi, P. (2005). When the social context frames the case: Counterfactuals in the courtroom. In D. Mandel, D. Hilton, P. Catellani, (Eds.), The psychology of counterfactual thinking (pp. 183–198). London, UK: Routledge. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Cislak, A. , & Wojciszke, B. (2008). Agency and communion are inferred from actions serving interests of self or others. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 1103–1110. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Douglas, K. M. , Sutton, R. M. (2003). Effects of communication goals and expectancies on language abstraction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 682–696. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Esposo, S. R. , Hornsey, M. J. , Spoor, J. R. (2013). Shooting the messenger: Outsiders critical of your group are rejected regardless of argument quality. British Journal of Social Psychology, 52, 386–395. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fiedler, K. (2008). Language – A toolbox for sharing and influencing social reality. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 38–47. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fiedler, K. , Mata, A. (2013). The art of exerting verbal influence through powerful lexical stimuli. In J. P. Forgas, J. Laszlo, O. Vincze, (Eds.), Social cognition and communication (pp. 43–62). New York, NY: Psychology Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Fischer, P. , Jonas, E. , Frey, D. , Schulz‐Hardt, S. (2005). Selective exposure to information: The impact of information limits. European Journal of Social Psychology, 35, 469–492. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fiske, S. T. (1980). Attention and weight in person perception: The impact of negative and extreme behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 889–906. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Funk, C. L. (1996). The impact of scandal on candidate evaluations: An experimental test of the role of candidate traits. Political Behavior, 18, 1–24. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gawronski, B. , Walther, E. (2008). The TAR effect: When the ones who dislike become the ones who are disliked. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 1276–1289. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Girotto, V. , Legrenzi, P. , Rizzo, A. (1991). Event controllability in counterfactual thinking. Acta Psychologica, 78, 111–133. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Haddock, G. , Zanna, M. (1997). Impact of negative advertising on evaluations of political candidates: The 1993 Canadian federal election. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 19, 205–223. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hill, R. P. (1989). An exploration of voter responses to political advertisements. Journal of Advertising, 18, 14–22. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hornsey, M. J. (2005). Why being right is not enough: Predicting defensiveness in the face of group criticism. European Review of Social Psychology, 16, 301–334. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hornsey, M. J. , Imani, A. (2004). Criticizing groups from the inside and the outside: An identity perspective on the intergroup sensitivity effect. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 365–383. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hornsey, M. J. , Oppes, T. , Svensson, A. (2002). ‘‘It’s ok if we say it, but you can’t’’: Responses to intergroup and intragroup criticism. European Journal of Social Psychology, 32, 293–307. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hornsey, M. J. , Robson, E. , Smith, J. , Esposo, S. , Sutton, R. M. (2008). Sugaring the pill: Assessing rhetorical strategies designed to minimize defensive reactions to group criticism. Human Communication Research, 34, 70–98. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ito, T. A. , Larsen, J. T. , Smith, N. K. , Cacioppo, J. T. (1998). Negative information weighs more heavily on the brain: The negativity bias in evaluative categorizations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 887–900. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kahneman, D. , Miller, D. T. (1986). Norm theory: Comparing reality to its alternatives. Psychological Review, 93, 136. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kahneman, D. , Tversky, A. (1982). The simulation heuristic. In D. Kahneman, E. Slovic, A. Tversky, (Eds.), Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases (pp. 201–208). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kervyn, N. , Dolderer, M. , Mahieu, T. , Yzerbyt, V. Y. (2010). Atypicality and the two fundamental dimensions: Applying the negativity effect on warmth to group perception. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 484–489. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Mandel, D. R. (2003). Effect of counterfactual and factual thinking on causal judgments. Thinking and Reasoning, 9, 245–265. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Markman, K. D. , Tetlock, P. E. (2000). “I couldn’t have known”: Accountability, foreseeability and counterfactual denials of responsibility. British Journal of Social Psychology, 39, 313–325. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Matthews, D. , Dietz-Uhler, B. (1998). The black-sheep effect: How positive and negative advertisements affect voters’ perceptions of the sponsor of the advertisement. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28, 1903–1915. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McClure, J. , Hilton, D. J. , Sutton, R. M. (2007). Judgments of voluntary and physical causes in causal chains: Probabilistic and social functionalist criteria for attributions. European Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 879–901. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nario-Redmond, M. , Branscombe, N. (1996). It could have been better or it might have been worse: Implications for blame assignment in rape cases. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 18, 347–366. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Peeters, G. (1971). The positive-negative asymmetry: On cognitive consistency and positivity bias. European Journal of Social Psychology, 1, 455–474. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Peeters, G. , Czapinski, J. (1990). Positive-negative asymmetry in evaluations: The distinction between affective and informational negativity effects. European Review of Social Psychology, 1, 33–60. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rabinovich, A. , Morton, T. A. (2010). Who says we are bad people? The impact of criticism source and attributional content on responses to group-based criticism. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 524–36. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Reeder, G. D. , Brewer, M. B. (1979). A schematic model of dispositional attribution in interpersonal perception. Psychological Review, 86, 61–79. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Roese, N. J. (1997). Counterfactual thinking. Psychological Bulletin, 121(1), 133–148. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Roese, N. J. , Sande, G. N. (1993). Backlash effect in attack politics. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 23, 632–653. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Skowronski, J. J. (2002). Honesty and intelligence judgments of individuals and groups: The effects of entity-related behaviour diagnosticity and implicit theories. Social Cognition, 20, 136–169. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Skowronski, J. J. , Carlston, D. E. (1987). Social judgment and social memory: The role of cue diagnosticity in negativity, positivity, and extremity biases. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 689–699. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Skowronski, J. J. , Carlston, D. E. (1989). Negativity and extremity biases in impression formation: A review of explanations. Psychological Bulletin, 105, 131–142. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Skowronski, J. J. , Carlston, D. E. , Mae, L. , Crawford, M. T. (1998). Spontaneous trait transference: Communicators take on the qualities they describe in others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 837–848. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sobel, M. E. (1982). Asymptotic intervals for indirect effects in structural equations models. In S. Leinhart, (Ed.), Sociological methodology (pp. 290–312). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Taber, C. S. , Lodge, M. (2006). Motivated skepticism in the evaluation of political beliefs. American Journal of Political Science, 50, 755–769. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tal-Or, N. , Boninger, D. S. , Poran, A. , Gleicher, F. (2004). Counterfactual thinking as a mechanism in narrative persuasion. Human Communication Research, 30, 301–328. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Thaler, V. (2012). Mitigation as modification of illocutionary force. Journal of Pragmatics, 44, 907–919. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vonk, R. (1999). Effects of other-profitability and self-profitability on evaluative judgements of behaviours. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 833–842. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wells, G. L. , Gavanski, I. (1989). Mental simulation of causality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 161–169. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wojciszke, B. (2005). Morality and competence in person and self-perception. European Review of Social Psychology, 16, 155–188. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wojciszke, B. , Abele, A. E. (2008). The primacy of communion over agency and its reversals in evaluations. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 1139–1147. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wojciszke, B. , Brycz, H. , Borkenau, P. (1993). Effects of information content and evaluative extremity on positivity and negativity biases. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 327–336. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wong, E. M. (2010). It could have been better: The effects of counterfactual communication on impression formation. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 1251–1260. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wyer, R. S. , Swan, S. , Gruenfeld, D. H. (1995). Impression formation in informal conversations. Social Cognition, 13, 243–272. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ybarra, O. , Chan, E. , Park, D. , Burnstein, E. , Monin, B. , Stanik, C. (2008). Life’s recurring challenges and the fundamental dimensions: An integration and its implications for cultural differences and similarities. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 1083–1092. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar