Skip to main content
Original Article

The Conspiracy Mentality Scale

Distinguishing Between Irrational and Rational Suspicion

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000381

Abstract. Beliefs in conspiracy theories, generally considered to be a unidimensional construct, are associated with negative outcomes. The existing measures of conspiracy theory beliefs have number of shortcomings. We present the development of a novel measure of the tendency to believe in conspiracy theories and report the discovery of a second factor that reflects rational skepticism. In Study 1 (N = 500) we use item response theory to devise the final items. In Study 2 (N = 202) we demonstrate the predictive validity of the two factors for different types of conspiracies. In Study 3 (N = 308) we demonstrate convergent/divergent validity. In Study 4 (N = 800) we demonstrate construct validity in three countries. Implications for the concept of conspiracy theory and conspiracy theory beliefs are discussed.

References

  • Abalakina-Paap, M., Stephan, W. G., Craig, T., & Gregory, W. L. (1999). Beliefs in conspiracies. Political Psychology, 20, 637–647. https://doi.org/10.1111/0162-895X.00160 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bale, J. M. (2007). Political paranoia v. political realism: On distinguishing between bogus conspiracy theories and genuine conspiratorial politics. Patterns of Prejudice, 41, 45–60. https://doi.org/10.1080/00313220601118751 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Barkun, M. (2003). A culture of conspiracy: Apocalyptic visions in contemporary America. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. https://doi.org/10.1525/california/9780520238053.001.0001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Basham, L. (2003). Malevolent global conspiracies. Journal of Social Philosophy, 34, 91–103. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9833.00167 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, 57, 289–300. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bjerg, O., & Presskorn-Thygesen, T. (2017). Conspiracy theory: Truth claim or language game. Theory Culture & Society, 34, 137–159. https://doi.org/10.1177/0263276416657880 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brotherton, R. (2013). Towards a definition of “conspiracy theory”. PsyPAG Quarterly, 88, 9–14. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Brotherton, R. (2015). Suspicious minds: Why we believe conspiracy theories. New York, NY: Bloomsbury Sigma. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Brotherton, R., & Eser, S. (2015). Bored to fears: Boredom proneness, paranoia, and conspiracy theories. Personality and Individual Differences, 80, 1–5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.02.011 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brotherton, R., French, C., & Pickering, A. (2013). Measuring belief in conspiracy theories: The generic conspiracist beliefs scale. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 279–282. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00279 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bruder, M., Haffke, P., Neave, N., Nouripanah, N., & Imhoff, R. (2013). Measuring individual differences in generic beliefs in conspiracy theories across cultures: Conspiracy mentality questionnaire. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 225. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00225 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Byford, J. (2011). Conspiracy theories: A critical introduction. Basingstoke, UK: Palgrave MacMillan. https://doi.org/10.1057/9780230349216 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Byrne, B. M. (2009). Structural equation modeling with AMOS: Basic concepts, applications, and programming (2nd ed.). Retrieved from https://ebookcentral-proquest-com First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Cai, L., Maydeu-Olivares, A., Coffman, D. L., & Thissen, D. (2006). Limited information goodness-of-fit testing of item response theory models for sparse 2p tables. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 59, 173–194. https://doi.org/10.1348/000711005X66419 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cai, L., Thissen, D., & du Toit, S. H. C. (2017). IRTPRO 4.1 for Windows [Computer software]. Skokie, IL: Scientific Software International. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Cichocka, A., Marchlewska, M., Golec de Zavala, A., & Olechowski, M. (2015). Grandiose delusions. Collective narcissism, secure in-group identification, and belief in conspiracies. In M. BilewiczA. CichochkaW. SoralEds., The Psychology of Conspiracy. London, UK: Routledge. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Cichocka, A., Marchlewska, M., Golec de Zavala, A., & Olechowski, M. (2016). “They will not control us”: Ingroup positivity and belief in intergroup conspiracies. British Journal of Psychology, 107, 556–576. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12158 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chen, F. (2007). Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling, 14, 464–504. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705510701301834 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cheung, G. W., & Rensvold, R. B. (2002). Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling, 9, 233–255. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15328007SEM0902_5 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Coady, D. (2007a). Conspiracy theories. Episteme, 42, 131–134. https://doi.org/10.1353/epi.2007.0019 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Coady, D. (2007b). Are conspiracy theorists irrational? Episteme, 42, 193–204. https://doi.org/10.3366/epi.2007.4.2.193 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Conte, H. R., Weiner, M. B., & Plutchik, R. (1982). Measuring death anxiety: Conceptual, psychometric, and factor-analytic aspects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 775–785. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.43.4.775 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cortina, J. (1993). What is coefficient alpha: an examination of theory and applications. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 98–104. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.78.1.98 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cubitt, G. (1989). Conspiracy myths and conspiracy theories. Journal of the Anthropological Society of Oxford, 20, 12–26. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Cubitt, G. (1993). The Jesuit myth: Conspiracy theory and politics in nineteenth-century France. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dagnall, N., Denovan, A., Drinkwater, K., Parker, A., & Clough, P. (2017). Statistical bias and endorsement of conspiracy theories. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 31, 368–378. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3331 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Darwin, H., Neave, N., & Holmes, J. (2011). Belief in conspiracy theories. The role of paranormal belief, paranoid ideation and schizotypy. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 1289–1293. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.02.027 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • deHaven-Smith, L. (2010). Beyond conspiracy theory: Patterns of high crime in American government. American Behavioral Scientist, 53, 795–825. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002764209353274 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • deHaven-Smith, L., & Witt, M. T. (2012). Conspiracy theory reconsidered responding to mass suspicions of political criminality in high office. Administration and Society, 45, 267–295. https://doi.org/10.1177/0095399712459727 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Demerens, F., Pare, J.-L., & Redis, J. (2013). Investor scepticism and creative accounting: The case of a French SME listed on Alternext. International Journal of Business, 18, 59–80. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Dentith, M. (2014). The philosophy of conspiracy theories. New York, NY: Palgrave Macmillan. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Drinkwater, K., Dagnall, N., & Parker, A. (2012). Reality testing, conspiracy theory, and paranormal beliefs. The Journal of Parapsychology, 76, 57–77. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Duran, N. D., Nicholson, S. P., & Dale, R. (2017). The hidden appeal and aversion to political conspiracies as revealed in the response dynamics of partisans. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 73, 268–278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2017.07.008 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ender, P. (n.d.). FAPARA.HLP.HTM. Retrieved from https://stats.idre.ucla.edu/stata/ado/analysis/fapara-hlp-htm/ First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Fekete, L. (2011). The Muslim conspiracy theory and the Oslo massacre. Race and Class, 53, 30–47. https://doi.org/10.1177/0306396811425984 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Feningstein, A., & Vanable, P. A. (1992). Paranoia and self-consciousness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 129–138. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.62.1.129 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fenster, M. (1999). Conspiracy theories: Secrecy and power in American culture. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Field, A. (2009). Discovering statistics using SPSS. London, UK: SAGE. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ford, G. T., Smith, D. B., & Swasy, J. L. (1990). Consumer scepticism of advertising claims: Testing hypotheses from economics od information. Journal of Consumer Research, 16, 433–441. https://doi.org/10.1086/209228 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Forehand, M. R., & Grier, S. (2003). When is honesty the best policy? The effect of stated company intent on consumer scepticism. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 13, 349–356. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327663JCP1303_15 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Galliford, N., & Furnham, A. (2017). Individual difference factors and beliefs in medical and political conspiracy theories. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 58, 422–428. https://doi.org/10.1111/sjop.12382 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Goertzel, T. (1994). Belief in conspiracy theories. Political Psychology, 15, 731–742. https://doi.org/10.2307/3791630 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gosling, S. D., Rentfrow, P. J., & Swann, W. B. Jr. (2003). A very brief measure of the big five personality domains. Journal of Research in Personality, 37, 504–528. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0092-6566(03)00046-1 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Green, R., & Douglas, K. (2018). Anxious attachment and belief in conspiracy theories. Personality and Individual Differences, 125, 30–37. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.12.023 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hagen, K. (2018). Conspiracy theories and the paranoid style: Do conspiracy theories posit implausibly vast and evil conspiracies? Social Epistemology, 32, 24–40. https://doi.org/10.1080/02691728.2017.1352625 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hofstadter, R. (1966). The paranoid style in American politics. In R. HofstadterEd., The paranoid style in American politics and other essays (pp. 3–40). New York, NY: Knopf. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Horn, J. L. (1965). A rationale and test for the number of factors in factor analysis. Psychometrika, 30, 179–185. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02289447 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hurtt, R. K. (2010). Development of a scale to measure professional scepticism. Auditing: A Journal of Practice & Theory, 29, 149–171. https://doi.org/10.2308/aud.2010.29.1.149 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Husting, G., & Orr, M. (2007). Dangerous machinery: “Conspiracy theorist” as a transpersonal strategy of exclusion. Symbolic Interaction, 30, 127–150. https://doi.org/10.1525/si.2007.30.2.127 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Imhoff, R., & Bruder, M. (2014). Speaking (un-)truth to power: Conspiracy Mentality as a generalized political attitude. European Journal of Personality, 28, 25–43. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.1930 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Imhoff, R., & Lamberty, P. K. (2017). Too special to be duped: Need for uniqueness motivates conspiracy beliefs. European Journal of Social Psychology, 47, 724–734. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2265 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Imhoff, R., & Lamberty, P. (2018). How paranoid are conspiracy believers? Toward a more fine‐grained understanding of the connect and disconnect between paranoia and belief in conspiracy theories. European Journal of Social Psychology, 48, 909–926. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2494 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Internal Revenue Service. (2017a). Examples of public corruption investigations for the fiscal year 2016. Retrieved from https://www.irs.gov/compliance/criminal-investigation/examples-of-public-corruption-investigations-fiscal-year-2016 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Internal Revenue Service. (2017b). Examples of public corruption investigations for the fiscal year 2015. Retrieved from https://www.irs.gov/compliance/criminal-investigation/examples-of-public-corruption-investigations-fiscal-year-2015 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Jane, E., & Fleming, C. (2014). Modern conspiracy: The Importance of being paranoid. New York, NY: Bloomsbury Publishing. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Jensen, T. (2013). Democrats and Republicans differ on conspiracy theory beliefs. Retrieved from https://www.publicpolicypolling.com/polls/democrats-and-republicans-differ-on-conspiracy-theory-beliefs/ First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Jensen, T. (2016). GOP quickly unifies around Trump; Clinton still has modest lead. Retrieved from https://www.publicpolicypolling.com/polls/gop-quickly-unifies-around-trump-clinton-still-has-modest-lead/ First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Jolley, D., & Douglas, K. M. (2014a). The effects of anti-vaccine conspiracy theories on vaccination intentions. PLoS One, 9, e89177. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0089177 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jolley, D., & Douglas, K. M. (2014b). The social consequences of conspiracism: Exposure to conspiracy theories decreases the intention to engage in politics and to reduce one’s carbon footprint. British Journal of Psychology, 105, 35–56. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12018 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Knight, P. (2000). Conspiracy culture: From Kennedy to the X-files. London, UK: Routledge. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kreko, P. (2015). Conspiracy theory as collective motivated cognition. In M. BilewiczA. CichochkaW. SoralEds., The psychology of conspiracy theories. London, UK: Routledge. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Lantian, A., Muller, D., Nurra, C., & Douglas, K. M. (2016). Measuring belief in conspiracy theories: Validation of a French and English Single-Item Scale. International Review of Social Psychology, 29, 1–14. https://doi.org/10.5334/irsp.8 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lantian, A., Muller, D., Nurra, C., & Douglas, K. M. (2017). “I know things they don’t know!” The role of need for uniqueness in belief in conspiracy theories. Social Psychology, 48, 160–173. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000306 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Lantian, A., Muller, D., Nurra, C., Klein, O., Berjot, S., & Pantazi, M. (2018). Stigmatized beliefs: Conspiracy theories, anticipated negative evaluation of the self, and fear of social exclusion. European Journal of Social Psychology, 48, 939–954. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2498 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Leone, L., Giacomantonio, M., & Lauriola, M. (2017). Moral foundations, worldviews, moral absolutism and belief in conspiracy theories. International Journal of Psychology, 54, 197–204. https://doi.org/10.1002/ijop.12459 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Leone, L., Giacomantonio, M., Williams, R., & Michetti, D. (2018). Avoidant attachment style and conspiracy theories. Personality and Individual Differences, 134, 329–336. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.06.043 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lobato, E., Mendoza, J., Sims, V., & Chin, M. (2014). Examining the relationship between conspiracy theories, paranormal beliefs, and pseudoscience acceptance among a university population. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28, 617–625. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3042 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mashiri, A., & Zaduqisti, E. (2014). We believe in your conspiracy if we distrust you: The role of intergroup distrust in structuring the effect of Islamic identification, competitive victimhood, and group incompatibility on belief in conspiracy theory. Journal of Tropical Psychology, 4, E11. https://doi.org/10.1017/jtp.2014.11 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mashuri, A., & Zaduqisti, E. (2015). The effect of intergroup threat and social identity salience on the belief in conspiracy theories over terrorism in Indonesia: Collective angst as mediator. International Journal of Psychological Research, 8, 24–35. https://doi.org/10.21500/20112084.642 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Maydeu-Olivares, A., & Joe, H. (2005). Limited and full information estimation and testing in 2n contingency tables: A unified framework. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 100, 1009–1020. https://doi.org/10.1198/016214504000002069 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Maydeu-Olivares, A., & Joe, H. (2006). Limited information goodness-of-fit testing in multidimensional contingency tables. Psychometrika, 71, 713–732. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11336-005-1295-9 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McCauley, C., & Jacques, S. (1979). The popularity of conspiracy theories of presidential assassination: A Bayesian analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 637–644. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.37.5.637 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mercier, H. (2017). How gullible are we? A review of the evidence from psychology and social science. Review of General Psychology, 21, 103–122. https://doi.org/10.1037/gpr0000111 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mohr, L. A., Eroǧlu, D., & Ellen, P. S. (1998). The development and testing of a measure of skepticism toward environmental claims in marketers’ communications. Journal of Consumer Affairs, 32, 30–55. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6606.1998.tb00399.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Newheiser, A.-K., Farias, M., & Tausch, N. (2011). The functional nature of conspiracy beliefs: Examining the underpinnings of belief in the Da Vinci Code conspiracy. Personality and Individual Differences, 51, 1007–1011. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.08.011 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Obermiller, C., & Spangenberg, E. R. (1998). Development of a scale to measure consumer skepticism toward advertising. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 7, 159–186. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327663jcp0702_03 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Oliver, E. J., & Wood, T. J. (2014). Conspiracy theories and the paranoid style(s) of mass opinion. American Journal of Political Science, 58, 952–966. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajps.12084 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Olmsted, K. (2009). Real enemies: Conspiracy theories and American democracy, World War I to 9/11. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Orlando, M., & Thissen, D. (2000). Likelihood-based item fit indices for dichotomous item response theory models. Applied Psychological Measurement, 24, 50–64. https://doi.org/10.1177/01466216000241003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Orlando, M., & Thissen, D. (2003). Further investigation of the performance of S-χ2: An item fit index for use with dichotomous item response theory models. Applied Psychological Measurement, 27, 289–298. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146621603027004004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Orosz, G., Krekó, P., Paskuj, B., Tóth-Király, I., Bőthe, B., & Roland-Lévy, C. (2016). Changing conspiracy beliefs through rationality and ridiculing. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1525. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01525 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ortmann, S., & Heathershaw, J. (2012). Conspiracy theories in the post‐soviet space. The Russian Review, 71, 551–564. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9434.2012.00668.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pacini, R., & Epstein, S. (1999). The relation of rational and experiential information processing styles to personality, basic beliefs, and the ratio-bias phenomena. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 972–987. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.972 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Peirce, J. W. (2007). PsychoPy – psychophysics software in Python. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 162, 8–13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2006.11.017 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pigden, C. (2006). Complots of mischief. In D. CoadyEd., Conspiracy theories: The philosophical debate (pp. 139–166). Aldershot, UK: Ashgate. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Pipes, D. (1996). The hidden hand: Middle East fears of conspiracy. New York, NY: St. Martin’s Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Pipes, D. (1997). How the paranoid style flourishes and where it comes form. New York, NY: Free Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Robins, R., & Post, J. (1997). Political paranoia: The psychopolitics of hatred. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Samejima, F. (1969). Estimation of a latent ability using a response pattern of graded scores. Psychometrika Monograph Supplement, 34. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2333-8504.1968.tb00153.x First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Schmitt, N. (1996). Uses and abuses of coefficient alpha. Psychological Assessment, 8, 350–353. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.8.4.350 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sijtsma, K. (2009). On the use, the misuse, and the very limited usefulness of Cronbach’s Alpha. Psychometrika, 74, 107–120. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11336-008-9101-0 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sperber, D., Clement, F., Heintz, C., Mascaro, O., Mercier, H., Origgi, G., & Wilson, D. (2010). Epistemic vigilance. Mind & Language, 25, 359–393. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0017.2010.01394.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sunstein, C. R., & Vermeule, A. (2009). Conspiracy theories: Causes and cures. Journal of Political Philosophy, 17, 202–227. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9760.2008.00325.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Swami, V., & Furnham, A. (2011). Preliminary examination of the psychometric properties of the Psychiatric Scepticism Scale. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 52, 399–403. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.2011.00881.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Swami, V., & Furnham, A. (2012). Examining conspiracist beliefs about the disappearance of Amelia Earhart. Journal of General Psychology, 139, 244–259. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.2012.697932 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Swami, V., Barron, D., Weis, L., Voracek, M., Stieger, S., & Furnham, A. (2017). An examination of the factorial and convergent validity of four measures of conspiracist ideation, with recommendations for researchers. PLoS One, 12, e0172617. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0172617 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Swami, V., Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Furnham, A. (2010). Unanswered questions: A preliminary investigation of personality and individual difference predictors of 9/11 conspiracist beliefs. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24, 749–761. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1583 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Swami, V., Coles, R., Stieger, S., Pietschnig, J., Furnham, A., Rehim, S., & Voracek, M. (2011). Conspiracist ideation in Britain and Austria: Evidence of a monological belief system and associations between individual psychological differences and real-world and fictitious conspiracy theories. British Journal of Psychology, 102, 443–463. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.2010.02004.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Swami, V., Pietschnig, J., Tran, U. S., Nader, I., Stieger, S., & Voracek, M. (2013). Lunar lies: The impact of informational framing and individual differences in shaping conspiracist beliefs about the moon landings. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 27, 71–80. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.2873 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Swami, V., Voracek, M., Stieger, S., Tran, U., & Furnham, A. (2014). Analytic thinking reduces beliefs in conspiracy theories. Cognition, 133, 572–585. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.08.006 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Swami, V., Weis, L., Lay, A., Barron, D., & Furnham, A. (2016). Associations between belief in conspiracy theories and the maladaptive personality traits of the personality inventory for DSM-5. Psychiatry Research, 236, 86–90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2015.12.027 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tobacyk, J. (2004). A revised paranormal belief scale. The International Journal of Transpersonal Studies, 23, 94–98. https://doi.org/10.1037/t14015-000 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tsfati, Y. (2003). Does audience skepticism of the media matter in agenda setting? Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 47, 157–176. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15506878jobem4702_1 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Uenal, F. (2016). The “secret islamization” of Europe: Exploring integrated threat theory for predicting islamophobic conspiracy stereotypes. International Journal of Conflict and Violence, 10, 94–108. https://doi.org/10.4119/UNIBI/ijcv.499 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Uscinski, J. E., & Parent, J. M. (2014). American conspiracy theories. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199351800.001.0001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • van Prooijen, J.-W., & van Lange, P. A. M. (2014). The social dimension of belief in conspiracy theories. In J.-W. van ProoijenP. A. M. van LangeEds., Power, politics, and paranoia: Why people are suspicious of their leaders (pp. 237–253). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • van Prooijen, J.-W., & Acker, M. (2015). The influence of control on belief in conspiracy theories: Conceptual and applied extensions. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 29, 753–761. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3161 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wilson, M. W., & Rose, C. (2014). The role of paranoia in a dual-process motivational model of conspiracy belief. In J.-W. van ProoijenP. A. M. van LangeEds., Power politics and paranoia: Why people are suspicious of their leaders (pp. 273–291). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Wong, C.-S., & Law, K. S. (2002). The effects of leader and follower emotional intelligence on performance and attitude: An exploratory study. The Leadership Quarterly, 13, 243–274. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1048-9843(02)00099-1 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wood, M. J. (2016). Some dare call it conspiracy: Labeling something a conspiracy theory does not reduce belief in it. Political Psychology, 37, 695–705. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12285 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wood, M. J. (2017). Conspiracy suspicions as a proxy for beliefs in conspiracy theories: Implications for theory and measurement. British Journal of Psychology, 108, 507–527. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12231 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wood, M. J., Douglas, K. M., & Sutton, R. M. (2012). Dead and alive: Beliefs in contradictory conspiracy theories. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3, 767–773. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550611434786 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zhang, X. J., Ko, M., & Carpenter, D. (2016). Development of a scale to measure skepticism toward electronic word-of-mouth. Computers in Human Behavior, 56, 198–208. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.11.042 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar