Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1866-5888/a000002

Efficient self-regulation has been argued to consist of more than just setting goals and tenaciously pursuing them – it also requires that people adapt their goals to changing circumstances. Although previous studies have already focused on interindividual differences in goal disengagement (one aspect of goal adaptation), so far, no study has looked at predictors and consequences of daily work goal adaptation. As predicted, daily goal adaptation was related to the amount of unplanned tasks and the extent to which the time needed for tasks was underestimated. However, unlike previous research on goal disengagement, daily goal adaptation had a negative (and not a positive) effect on well-being and subjective productivity. It is suggested that the emotional aspect of goal adaptation/goal disengagement needs more research attention.

References

  • Ashkanasy, N. , Gupta, V. , Mayfield, M. S. , Trevor-Roberts, E. (2004). Future orientation. In R. J. House, P. J. Hanges, M. Javidan, P. W. Dorfman, V. Gupta (Eds.), Culture, leadership, and organizations: The GLOBE study of 62 societies (pp. 282–342). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Brandtstädter, J. , Renner, G. (1990). Tenacious goal pursuit and flexible goal adjustment: Explication and age-related analysis of assimilative and accommodative strategies of coping. Psychology and Aging, 5, 58–67. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Buehler, R. , Griffin, D. (2003). Planning, personality, and prediction: The role of future focus in optimistic time predictions. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 92, 80–90. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Buehler, R. , Griffin, D. , & Ross, M. (2002). Inside the planning fallacy: The causes and consequences of optimistic time predictions. In T. D. Gilovich, D. Griffin, D. Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment (pp. 250–270). New York: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Byram, S. J. (1997). Cognitive and motivational factors influencing time prediction. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 3, 216–239. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54, 493–503. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • González-Romá, V. , Espejo, B. (2003). Testing the middle response categories “not sure”, “in between” and “?” in polytomous items. Psicothema, 15, 278–284. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Heckhausen, J. , Wrosch, C. , Fleeson, W. (2001). Developmental regulation before and after a developmental deadline: The sample case of “biological clock” for childbearing. Psychology and Aging, 16, 400–413. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Henderson, M. D. , Gollwitzer, P. M. , Oettingen, G. (2007). Implementation intentions and disengagement from a failing course of action. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 20, 81–102. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ilies, R. , Scott, B. A. , Judge, T. A. (2006). The interactive effects of personal traits and experienced states on intraindividual patterns of citizenship behavior. Academy of Management Journal, 49, 561–575. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • König, C. J. (2005). Anchors distort estimates of expected duration. Psychological Reports, 96, 253–256. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Koole, S. , van’t Spijker, M. (2000). Overcoming the planning fallacy through willpower: Effects of implementation intentions on actual and predicted task-completion times. European Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 873–888. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Locke, E. A. , Latham, G. C. (1990). A theory of goal setting and task performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Louro, M. J. , Pieters, R. , Zeelenberg, M. (2007). Dynamics of multiple-goal pursuit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 174–193. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lozano, L. M. , García-Cueto, E. , Muñiz, J. (2008). Effect of the number of response categories on the reliability and validity of rating scales. Methodology, 4, 73–79. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Luong, A. , Rogelberg, S. G. (2005). Meetings and more meetings: The relationship between meeting load and the daily well-being of employees. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 9, 58–67. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mark, G. , González, V. M. , Harris, J. (2005). No task left behind?: Examining the nature of fragmented work. SIGCHI conference on human factors in computing systems (pp. 321–330). Portland, OR: ACM Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McNair, D. M. , Lorr, M. , & Droppelman, L. F. (1971). EITS Manual of the Profile of Mood States. San Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Service. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Miller, G. E. , Wrosch, C. (2007). You’ve gotta know when to fold ‘em: Goal disengagement and systemic inflammation in adolescence. Psychological Science, 18, 773–777. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Newby-Clark, I. R. , Ross, M. , Buehler, R. , Koehler, D. J. , Griffin, D. (2000). People focus on optimistic scenarios and disregard pessimistic scenarios while predicting task completion times. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 6, 171–182. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Oshagbemi, T. (1995). Management development and managers’ use of their time. Journal of Management Development, 14(8), 19–34. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Podsakoff, P. M. , MacKenzie, S. B. , Lee, J. Y. , Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 879–903. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pulakos, E. D. , Arad, S. , Donovan, M. A. , Plamondon, K. E. (2000). Adaptability in the workplace: Development of a taxonomy of adaptive performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 612–624. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Raudenbush, S. W. , Bryk, A. S. (2001). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Raudenbush, S. W. , Bryk, A. S. , & Congdon, R. T., Jr. (2006). HLM6.03: Hierarchical linear and nonlinear modeling [Computer software]. Chicago, IL: Scientific Software International. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Reeve, J. (2005). Understanding motivation and emotion (4th ed.). New York: Wiley. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Shah, J. Y. (2005). The automatic pursuit and management of goals. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 10–13. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sonnentag, S. , Natter, E. (2004). Flight attendants’ daily recovery from work: Is there no place like home? International Journal of Stress Management, 11, 366–391. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Spector, P. E. (2006). Method variance in organizational research: Truth or urban legend? Organizational Research Methods, 9, 221–232. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Thomas, K. E. , Handley, S. J. , Newstead, S. E. (2007). The role of prior task experience in temporal misestimation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 230–240. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wagenaar, W. A. (1986). My memory: A study of autobiographical memory over six years. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 225–252. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wrosch, C. , Heckhausen, J. (1999). Control processes before and after passing a developmental deadline: Activation and deactivation of intimate relationship goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 415–427. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wrosch, C. , Miller, G. E. , Scheier, M. F. , de Pontet, S. B. (2007). Giving up on unattainable goals: Benefits for health? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 251–265. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wrosch, C. , Scheier, M. F. , Carver, C. S. , Schulz, R. (2003). The importance of goal disengagement in adaptive self-regulation: When giving up is beneficial. Self and Identity, 2, 1–20. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wrosch, C. , Scheier, M. F. , Miller, G. E. , Schulz, R. , Carver, C. S. (2003). Adaptive self-regulation of unattainable goals: Goal disengagement, goal reengagement, and subjective well-being. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 1494–1508. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar