Skip to main content
Free AccessEditorial

Introduction to the Special Issue

Capturing the Dynamics of Emotion and Emotion Regulation in Daily Life With Ambulatory Assessment

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000599

In daily life, emotions fluctuate continuously in response to many factors, including appraisals of events, biological rhythms, and self-regulation (Kuppens & Verduyn, 2017). Furthermore, the relations among emotions and these factors are bidirectional, resulting in complex dynamics (Cunningham, Dunfield, & Stillman, 2013; Hollenstein, 2015). For instance, appraising an event as incompatible with one’s goals may trigger anger. In turn, this anger may be evaluated as situationally inappropriate prompting regulation efforts. And, if successful, regulation will influence the trajectory of anger over time (Gross, 2015). Thus, emotion generation influences emotion regulation and vice versa (Yih, Uusberg, Taxer, & Gross, 2019). This presents a formidable challenge for researchers seeking to understand dynamic emotional processes. Addressing this challenge requires valid and reliable measurement and modeling tools, which are still lacking in psychology (Borsboom, 2006; Flake & Fried, 2019) and in emotion research specifically (Brose, Schmiedek, Gerstorf, & Voelkle, 2019; Weidman, Steckler, & Tracy, 2017).

In particular, to capture the dynamics of emotion and emotion regulation in daily life, we need new intensive longitudinal assessment and modeling techniques. Although intensive longitudinal assessment of emotion can be traced back to Flügle’s seminal (1925) paper, it only began to flourish in the 1980s when the experience sampling method (ESM; Csikszentmihalyi & Larson, 1987), ecological momentary assessment (EMA; Stone & Shiffman, 1994), and related diary methods (Bolger, Davis, & Rafaeli, 2003) were developed. Advances in handheld computing (e.g., PDAs, smartphones) led to an explosion in daily life research in the 2000s, using what are now collectively referred to as ambulatory assessment (AA) methods (Trull & Ebner-Priemer, 2014).1

Several previous special issues, including one in this journal (Westmeyer, 2007) have covered AA (e.g., Kubiak & Stone, 2012; Schimmack & Diener, 2003) and its application to diverse subfields, including clinical psychology (Trull & Ebner-Priemer, 2009), addiction (Tomko & McClure, 2018), health psychology (Shiffman & Stone, 1998), and adolescent development (Modecki, Goldberg, Ehrenreich, Russell, & Bellmore, 2019). These special issues highlight the strengths of AA methods: capturing dynamic psychological processes in everyday life, thus providing a different perspective than traditional laboratory and global/retrospective questionnaire methods. So, why the need for another special issue now on this topic in the European Journal of Psychological Assessment? The volume of AA research continues to rise,2 and a growing number of studies focus specifically on the dynamics of emotion and emotion regulation in daily life (Brose et al., 2019; Colombo et al., 2020). This Special Issue aims to address measurement and modeling challenges that are particular to the study of emotional processes in everyday life. In what follows, we briefly outline some of the major challenges facing this field and outline how the eight contributions to this Special Issue help to address these challenges.

Challenges for the Study of Emotion and Emotion Regulation Dynamics in Daily Life

Measurement Challenges

The generation and regulation of emotion are theorized to be mutually interacting dynamic processes (Gross, 2015; Yih et al., 2019). Yet, most widely used measures of emotion (e.g., Watson & Walker, 1996) and emotion regulation (e.g., Gross & John, 2003) are global self-report questionnaires developed to assess stable dispositions rather than moment-to-moment dynamics. Dynamic measures of emotion have been developed for the lab (e.g., Coan & Gottman, 2007; Ruef & Levenson, 2007), but these are not practical for AA studies conducted across longer timescales in naturalistic settings. Thus, a major challenge for emotion researchers is to develop valid tools to measure people’s momentary experience and regulation of emotions in daily life and to describe and interpret their dynamics. Four contributions to this Special Issue tackle such measurement-related issues.

First, Medland et al. report on the development and validation of a new scale assessing momentary use of emotion regulation strategies in daily life. The 12-item scale is designed to be sufficiently brief for AA research while also capturing a broad range of regulation strategies with two items per strategy. This represents an important advance on previous AA studies, which have either assessed emotion regulation strategies using single-item measures or focused on a very limited number of strategies (Colombo et al., 2020).

Second, Blanke et al. investigate whether momentary self-reports of emotion regulation obtained with AA can yield reliable indices of an individual’s average use and dynamic variability in emotion regulation strategies across everyday contexts. By adapting Fleeson’s (2001) density distribution approach, these authors provide a framework for characterizing relatively stable individual differences in patterns of momentary emotion regulation.

Third, English and Eldesouky address how to interpret dynamic variability in emotion regulation strategy use, which is often considered to index healthy or flexible emotion regulation (Aldao, Sheppes, & Gross, 2015). These authors highlight challenges in assessing situational features, which may have hampered attempts to document the theorized benefits of moment-to-moment variability in emotion regulation, and they suggest ways to improve the measurement of daily life contexts in future research.

Finally, MacCann et al., examine the predictive validity of global emotional intelligence measures for daily emotion dynamics, based on a meta-analysis and an original AA study. Their study systematically tests the hypothesized benefits of emotional intelligence for everyday emotional functioning, bringing a domain that is usually studied only at the trait level into a dynamic framework and bridging two previously separate literatures.

Modeling Challenges

Understanding how people experience and regulate their emotions in daily life requires not only valid and reliable measurement tools, but also data analytic methods capable of capturing the complex dynamics and interplay among these processes. The other four contributions to this Special Issue develop or apply novel modeling approaches.

Van de Maat et al. highlight the importance of understanding and accounting for cyclical (e.g., diurnal) patterns in AA data on emotions. Although previous research has documented cyclical patterns in everyday emotions (e.g., Peeters, Berkhof, Delespaul, Rottenberg, & Nicolson, 2006), the detection of such cycles is often neglected in AA studies. Van de Maat et al. present an accessible overview of different methods for detecting and dealing with cycles in intensive sampling of emotion.

Another major challenge for researchers studying emotion dynamics concerns the identification of individual differences (Kuppens & Verduyn, 2017). To help address this challenge, Ernst et al. present a new modeling approach to induce latent classes of individuals with similar multivariate patterns of emotion dynamics, known as latent class vector-autoregressive modeling. This powerful analytic tool allows researchers to identify potentially novel emotional traits based on dynamic patterns in AA data, rather than relying solely on traditional (global/retrospective) self-report questionnaires as indicators of individual differences.

Ironically, while emotion dynamics is fundamentally about variability, most research in this area has (implicitly) assumed that parameters of emotion dynamics are themselves stable across contexts and time (but see, e.g., Dejonckheere et al., 2019; Koval & Kuppens, 2012). Albers and Bringmann provide researchers with a powerful tool for moving beyond this “stationarity” assumption by presenting a novel technique for identifying gradual and abrupt changes in emotion dynamics using time-varying change-point autoregressive models.

Finally, Bosley et al. apply a novel idiographic modeling approach to investigate reciprocal associations among positive affect and worry in daily life, testing the predictions of a prominent model of anxiety. Their findings demonstrate that both the magnitude and direction of associations between positive affect and worry differ substantially across people. This implies that theoretical accounts of emotion and emotion regulation may need to account for substantial individual differences in the relations among these processes, highlighting the utility of idiographic modeling.

Conclusion

Taken together, the eight contributions to this Special Issue address some of the major assessment and modeling challenges facing researchers studying the dynamics of everyday emotional processes. Of course, there are many other challenges facing our field. For instance, given that research on emotional functioning in daily life relies heavily on self-report, we must do more to understand and mitigate the influences of measurement reactivity (Shrout et al., 2018) and response styles (Baird, Lucas, & Donnellan, 2017) and more work is needed to validate non-self-report methods for assessing implicit experience and regulation of emotion in daily life (Sarsenbayeva et al., 2020). Nevertheless, we hope this Special Issue presents a step forward in tackling the complex measurement challenges posed by studying emotions and emotion regulation in everyday life.

1We use the term Ambulatory Assessment to subsume both momentary self-reports (i.e., ESM/EMA/diary methods) and intensive sampling of behavior and physiology using mobile/wearable sensors in naturalistic settings, consistent with the definition of the Society for Ambulatory Assessment (SAA; http://ambulatory-assessment.org/).

2We conducted a Google Scholar search (on April 7, 2020) for publications with the terms “experience sampling,” “ecological momentary assessment,” “ambulatory assessment,” or “daily diary” in their title and found a steadily increasing number in each year from 2010 (n = 131) to 2019 (n = 518). This is merely illustrative of a broader trend as many more publications use AA methods but do not include one of these terms in their title.

References

  • Aldao, A., Sheppes, G., & Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion Regulation Flexibility. Cognitive Therapy Research, 39, 263–278. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-014-9662-4 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baird, B. M., Lucas, R. E., & Donnellan, M. B. (2017). The role of response styles in the assessment of intraindividual personality variability. Journal of Research in Personality, 69, 170–179. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2016.06.015 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bolger, N., Davis, A., & Rafaeli, E. (2003). Diary methods: Capturing life as it is lived. Annual Review Psychology, 54, 579–616. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145030 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Borsboom, D. (2006). The attack of the psychometricians. Psychometrika, 71, 425–440. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11336-006-1447-6 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brose, A., Schmiedek, F., Gerstorf, D., & Voelkle, M. C. (2019). The measurement of within-person affect variation. Emotion, 20, 677–699. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000583 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Coan, J. A., & Gottman, J. M. (2007). The Specific Affect Coding System (SPAFF). In J. A. CoanJ. J. B. AllenEds., Handbook of emotion elicitation and assessment (pp. 267–285). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Colombo, D., Fernández-Álvarez, J., Suso-Ribera, C., Cipresso, P., Valev, H., Leufkens, T., … Botella, C. (2020). The need for change: Understanding emotion regulation antecedents and consequences using ecological momentary assessment. Emotion, 20, 30–36. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000671 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Larson, R. (1987). Validity and reliability of the experience-sampling method. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 175, 526–536. https://doi.org/10.1097/00005053-198709000-00004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cunningham, W. A., Dunfield, K. A., & Stillman, P. E. (2013). Emotional states from affective dynamics. Emotion Review, 5, 344–355. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073913489749 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dejonckheere, E., Mestdagh, M., Verdonck, S., Lafit, G., Ceulemans, E., Bastian, B., & Kalokerinos, E. K. (2019). The relation between positive and negative affect becomes more negative in response to personally relevant events. Emotion. Advance online article. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000697 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Flake, J. K., & Fried, E. I. (2019). Measurement schmeasurement: Questionable measurement practices and how to avoid them. [Preprint]. Retrieved from PsyArXiv https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/hs7wm First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Fleeson, W. (2001). Toward a structure- and process-integrated view of personality: Traits as density distributions of states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 1011–1027. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.80.6.1011 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Flügel, J. C. (1925). A quantitative study of feeling and emotion in everyday life. British Journal of Psychology: General Section, 15, 318–355. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1925.tb00187.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation: Current status and future prospects. Psychological Inquiry, 26, 1–26. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2014.940781 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 348–362. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hollenstein, T. (2015). This time, it’s real: Affective flexibility, time scales, feedback loops, and the regulation of emotion. Emotion Review, 7, 308–315. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073915590621 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Koval, P., & Kuppens, P. (2012). Changing emotion dynamics: Individual differences in the effect of anticipatory social stress on emotional inertia. Emotion, 12, 256–267. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024756 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kubiak, T., & Stone, A. A. (2012). Ambulatory monitoring of biobehavioral processes in health and disease. Psychosomatic Medicine, 74, 325–326. https://doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0b013e31825878da First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kuppens, P., & Verduyn, P. (2017). Emotion dynamics. Current Opinion in Psychology, 17, 22–26. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.06.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Modecki, K. L., Goldberg, R. E., Ehrenreich, S. E., Russell, M., & Bellmore, A. (2019). The practicalities and perils of ambulatory assessment’s promise: Introduction to a special section. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 29, 542–550. https://doi.org/10.1111/jora.12532 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Peeters, F., Berkhof, J., Delespaul, P., Rottenberg, J., & Nicolson, N. A. (2006). Diurnal mood variation in major depressive disorder. Emotion, 6, 383–391. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.6.3.383 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ruef, A. M., & Levenson, R. W. (2007). Continuous measurement of emotion: The affect rating dial. In J. A. CoanJ. J. B. AllenEds., Handbook of emotion elicitation and assessment (pp. 286–297). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Sarsenbayeva, Z., Marini, G., van Berkel, N., Luo, C., Jiang, W., Yang, K., … Goncalves, J. (2020). Does smartphone use drive our emotions or vice versa? A causal analysis. Proceedings of the 2020 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, 1–15. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Schimmack, U., & Diener, E. (2003). Editorial. Journal of Happiness Studies, 4, 1–4. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023652821045 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Shiffman, S., & Stone, A. A. (1998). Introduction to the special section: Ecological momentary assessment in health psychology. Health Psychology, 17, 3–5. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0092706 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Shrout, P. E., Stadler, G., Lane, S. P., McClure, M. J., Jackson, G. L., Clavél, F. D., … Bolger, N. (2018). Initial elevation bias in subjective reports. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA, 115, E15–E23. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1712277115 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stone, A. A., & Shiffman, S. (1994). Ecological momentary assessment (EMA) in behavorial medicine. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 16, 199–202. https://doi.org/10.1093/abm/16.3.199 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tomko, R. L., & McClure, E. A. (2018). Introduction to the special issue: Utilizing ambulatory assessment to better understand the etiology, maintenance, treatment, and remission of addictive disorders. Addictive Behaviors, 83, 1–4. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2018.04.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Trull, T. J., & Ebner-Priemer, U. W. (2009). Using Experience Sampling Methods/Ecological Momentary Assessment (ESM/EMA) in clinical assessment and clinical research: Introduction to the Special Section. Psychological Assessment, 21, 457–462. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017653 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Trull, T. J., & Ebner-Priemer, U. (2014). The role of ambulatory assessment in psychological science. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23, 466–470. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721414550706 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Watson, D., & Walker, L. M. (1996). The long-term stability and predictive validity of trait measures of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 567–577. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.70.3.567 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Weidman, A. C., Steckler, C. M., & Tracy, J. L. (2017). The jingle and jangle of emotion assessment: Imprecise measurement, casual scale usage, and conceptual fuzziness in emotion research. Emotion, 17, 267–295. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000226 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Westmeyer, H. (2007). Introduction to the special issue on advances in the methodology of ambulatory assessment. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 23, 203–205. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759.23.4.203 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Yih, J., Uusberg, A., Taxer, J. L., & Gross, J. J. (2019). Better together: A unified perspective on appraisal and emotion regulation. Cognition and Emotion, 33, 41–47. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2018.1504749 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

Peter Koval, Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, VIC 3010, Australia, E-mail
Elise K. Kalokerinos, Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, VIC 3010, Australia, E-mail
Philippe Verduyn, Work and Social Psychology, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, 6200 MD Maastricht, The Netherlands, E-mail
Samuel Greiff, Cognitive Science and Assessment (COSA), University of Luxembourg, 2, avenue de l’Université, 4365 Esch sur Alzette, Luxembourg, E-mail