Skip to main content
Studien

Kreativität bei Kindern und Jugendlichen mit Asperger Syndrom

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1026/0942-5403/a000126

Das Ziel der vorliegenden Studie war die systematische Erfassung der Kreativitätsleistung bei 5- bis 14-jährigen Jungen mit Asperger Syndrom im Vergleich mit einer gesunden Kontrollgruppe (je n=24). Untersucht wurden sowohl quantitative Kreativitätsaspekte (Ideenflüssigkeit, Ideenflexibilität) als auch qualitative Kreativitätsaspekte (Originalität) bei zwei Altersgruppen (jüngere Kinder: 5 – 9 Jahre, ältere Kinder/Jugendliche: 10 – 14 Jahre). Die Ergebnisse zeigen, dass bei Kindern/Jugendlichen mit Asperger Syndrom primär die quantitativen Aspekte der Kreativität beeinträchtigt sind (Ideenflüssigkeit und -flexibilität bei gleichbleibenden Items aus dem 5-Punkte Test), während die Kreativitätsleistung bei den abwechslungsreicheren Bildergänzungsaufgaben des Torrance Test of Creative Thinking, bei denen die Qualität/Originalität im Vordergrund steht, unbeeinträchtigt ist. In unserer Stichproben waren die Alterseffekte signifikant (jüngere Kinder zeigten schlechtere Leistungen), aber über die Gruppen vergleichbar (d. h. die Interaktionseffekte waren nicht signifikant). Im Sinne einer ressourcenorientierten Diagnostik, die für eine maßgeschneiderte Interventionsplanung unerlässlich ist, sollten also zusätzlich zu den quantitativen auch die qualitativen Aspekte kreativer und exekutiver Denkleistungen erfasst werden.


Creativity in children and adolescents with Asperger syndrome

The aim of the present study was the systematic investigation of creativity in 5 to 14-year-old children/adolescents with Asperger syndrome compared to typically developing controls (each n = 24). Both quantitative (ideational fluency and flexibility) and qualitative creativity aspects (originality of generated ideas) in two age groups (5 to 9 year olds vs. 10 to14year olds) were examined. Our findings revealed that children/adolescents with Asperger syndrome compared with controls displayed impaired performance only on quantitative creativity aspects (i. e., ideational fluency and flexibility as indexed by the 5-point test) while more qualitative aspects of creativity were largely unimpaired (i. e., originality as indexed by the picture completion subtest of the Torrance tests of creative thinking [TTCT]). Moreover, age effects were significant in both groups (younger children performing worse) but were comparable across groups. Overall, our results are novel and clinically important as they indicate that creativity assessments of Asperger syndrome should include both quantitative and qualitative aspects of creativity.

Literatur

  • Ambery, F. Z. , Russell, A. J. , Perry, K. , Morris, R. & Murphy, D. G. (2006). Neuropsychological functioning in adults with Asperger syndrome. Autism, 10, 551 – 564. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Andreasen, N. C. (1987). Creativity and mental illness: Prevalence rates in writers and their first-degree relatives. American Journal of Psychiatry, 144, 1288 – 1292. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Barnard, L. , Muldoon, K. , Hasan, R. , O’Brien, G. , Stewart M., (2008). Profiling executive dysfunction in adults with autism and comorbid learning disability. Autism, 12, 125 – 141. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Best, J. R. , Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Developement, 81, 1641 – 1660. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bölte, S. , Poustka, F. (2004). The German form of the autism diagnostic observation schedule (ADOS): First results on reliability and validity. Zeitschrift für Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie und Psychotherapie, 32, 45 – 50. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Chermahini, S. A. , Hommel, B. (2010). The (b)link between creativity and dopamine: spontaneous eye blink rates predict and dissociate divergent and convergent thinking. Cognition, 115, 458 – 465. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Craig, J. , Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). Creativity and imagination in autism and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2, 319 – 326. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Craig, J. , Baron-Cohen, S. , Scott, F. (2001). Drawing ability in autism: A window into the imagination. Israel Journal of Psychiatry, 3, 242 – 253. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Dichter, G. S. , Lam, K. S. , Turner-Brown, L. M. , Holtzclaw, T. N. , Bodfish, J. W. (2009). Generativity abilities predict communication deficits but not repetitive behaviors in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Journal of Autism and Developemental Disorders, 39, 1298 – 1304. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dietrich, A. (2004). The cognitive neuroscience of creativity. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 11, 1011 – 1026. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dilling, H. , Mombour, W. & Schmidt, M. H. (Hrsg.). (2008). Internationale Klassifikation psychischer Störungen. ICD-10 Kapitel V (F). Klinisch-diagnostische Leitlinien (6., vollst. überarb. Aufl.). Bern: Huber First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Farah, M. J. , Haimm, C. , Sankoorikal, G. , Smith, M. E. , Chatterjee, A. (2009). When we enhance cognition with Adderall, do we sacrifice creativity? A preliminary study. Psychopharmacology, 202, 541 – 547. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Flaherty, A. W. (2011). Brain illness and creativity: mechanisms and treatment risks. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 56, 132 – 143. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Flaherty, A. W. (2005). Fronto-temporal and dopaminergic control of idea generation and creative drive. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 493, 147 – 153. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Frith, U. (1970). Studies in pattern detection in normal and autistic children. II. Reproduction and production of color sequences. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 10(1), 120 – 135. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Funk, J. B. , Chessare, J. B. , Weaver, M. T. , Exley, A. R. (1993). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, creativity, and the effects of methylphenidate. Pediatrics, 91, 816 – 819. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Geurts, H. M. , Corbett, B. , Solomon, M. (2009). The paradox of cognitive flexibility in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 74 – 82. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gioia, G. A. , Isquith, P. K. , Guy, S. C. , Kenworthy, L. (2000). Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function. Odessa: Psychological Assessment Resources. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Gotham, K. , Pickles, A. & Lord C., (2012). Trajectories of autism severity in children using standardized ADOS scores. Pediatrics, 130, 1278 – 1284 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Guilford, J. P. (1950). Creativity. American Psychologist, 5, 444 – 454. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Haid, T. , Martl, C. , Schubert, F. , Wenzl, M. , Kofler, M. , Saltuari, L. (2002). Der „HAMASCH 5 Punkt Test”–erste Normierungsergebnisse. Zeitschrift für Neuropsychologie, 13, 233. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hennessey, B. A. , Amabile, T. M. (2010). Creativity. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 569 – 598. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hobson, J. A. , Hobson, R. P. , Malik, S. , Bargiota, K. , Caló, S. (2013). The relation between social engagement and pretend play in autism. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 31, 114 – 127. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hughes, C. , Russell, J. & Robbins, T. W. (1994). Evidence for executive dysfunction in autism. Neuropsychologia, 32, 477 – 492. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jarrold, C. (2003). A review of research into pretend play in autism. Autism, 7, 379 – 390. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jauk, E. , Benedek, M. , Dunst, B. & Neubauer, A. C. (2013). The relationship between intelligence and creativity: New support for the threshold hypothesis by means of empirical breakpoint detection. Intelligence, 41, 212 – 221. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kaufmann, L. , Zotter, S. , Pixner, S. , Starke, M. , Haberlandt, E. , Steinmayr-Gensluckner, M. , Egger, K. , Schocke, M. et al. (2013). CANTAB performance and brain structure in pediatric patients with Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, 1483 – 1490. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kleinhans, N. , Akshoomoff, N. , Delis, D. C. (2005). Executive functions in autism and Asperger’s disorder: Flexibility, fluency, and inhibition. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27, 379 – 401. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kharitonova, M. , Martin, R. E. , Gabrieli, J. D. , Sheridan, M. A. (2013). Cortical gray-matter thinning is associated with age-related improvements on executive function tasks. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 61 – 71. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kyaga, S. , Lichtenstein, P. , Boman, M. , Hultman, C. , Långström, N. , Landén, M. (2011). Creativity and mental disorder: Family study of 300,000 people with severe mental disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry, 199, 373 – 379. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kyaga, S. , Landén, M. , Boman, M. , Hultman, C. M. , Långström, N. , Lichtenstein, P. (2013). Mental illness, suicide and creativity: 40-year prospective total population study. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 47, 83 – 90. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lopez, B. R. , Lincoln, A. J. , Ozonoff, S. & Lai, Z. (2005). Examining the relationship between executive functions and restricted, repetitive symptoms of autistic disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 445 – 460. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lord, C. , Rutter, M. , Couteur, A. L. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659 – 685. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lord, C. , Rutter, M. , Goode, S. (1989). Autism diagnostic observation schedule: A standardized observation of communicative and social behavior. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 185 – 212. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Low, J. , Goddard, E. , Melser, J. (2009). Generativity and imagination in autism spectrum disorder: Evidence from individual differences in children’s impossible entity drawings. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 27, 425 – 444. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mayseless, N. , Uzefovsky, F. , Shalev, I. , Ebstein, R. P. & Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. (2013). The association between creativity and 7R polymorphism in the dopamine receptor D4 gene (DRD4). Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 502. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ozonoff, S. , Jensen, J. (1999). Brief report: Specific executive function profiles in three neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 171 – 177. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pellicano, E. (2010). The development of core cognitive skills in autism: A 3-year prospective study. Child Development, 81, 1400 – 1416. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pellicano, E. (2012). The development of executive function in autism. Autism Research and Treatment, 2012, 146132. doi: 10.1155/2012/146132 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pellicano, E. (2013). Testing the predictive power of cognitive atypicalities in autistic children: Evidence from a 3-year follow-up study. Autism Research, 6, 258 – 267. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Poustka, F. , Lisch, S. , Ruhl, D. , Sacher, A. , Schmotzer, G. , Werner, K. (1996). The standardized diagnosis of autism: Autism diagnostic interview-revised: Interrater reliability of the German form of the interview. Psychopathology, 29, 145 – 153. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pring, L. , Ryder, N. , Crane, L. , Hermelin, B. (2012). Creativity in savant artists with autism. Autism, 16, 45 – 57. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Regard, M. , Strauss, E. & Knapp, P. (1982). Children′s production on verbal and nonverbal fluency tasks. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 55, 839 – 844. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rosenthal, M. , Wallace, G. L. , Lawson, R. , Wills, M. C. , Dixon, E. , Yerys, B. E. , Kenworthy, L. (2013). Impairments in real-world executive function increase from childhood to adolescence in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychology, 27, 13 – 18. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rumsey, J. M. , Duara, R. , Grady, C. , Rapoport, J. L. , Margolin, R. A. , Rapoport, S. I. , Cutler, N. R. (1985). Brain metabolism in autism. Resting cerebral glucose utilization rates as measured with positron emission tomography. Archives of General Psychiatry, 42, 448 – 455. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Runco, M. A. (2004). Creativity. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 657 – 687. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rutherford, M. D. , Rogers, S. J. (2003). Cognitive underpinnings of pretend play in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 289 – 302. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schott, G. D. (2012). Pictures as a neurological tool: Lessons from enhanced and emergent artistry in brain disease. Brain, 135, 1947 – 1963 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Scott, F. , Baron-Cohen, S. (1996). Imagining real and unreal things: Evidence of a dissociation in autism. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, 371 – 382. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Selemon, L. D. (2013). A role for synaptic plasticity in the adolescent development of executive function. Translational Psychiatry, 3:e238. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Semrud-Clikeman, M. , Walkowiak, J. , Wilkinson, A. , Butcher, B. (2010). Executive functioning in children with Asperger syndrome, ADHD-combined type, ADHD-predominantly inattentive type, and controls. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 1017 – 1027. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Semrud-Clikeman, M. , Fine, J. G. , Bledsoe, J. (2013). Comparison among children with children with autism spectrum disorder, nonverbal learning disorder and typically developing children on measures of executive functioning. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Epub ahead of print. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. , Adler, N. , Aharon-Peretz, J. , Perry, D. , Mayseless, N. (2011). The origins of originality: The neural basis of creative thinking and originality. Neuropsychologia, 49, 178 – 185. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sinzig, J. , Bruning, N. , Morsch, D. & Lehmkuhl, G. (2007). Altersabhängige Unterschiede in neuropsychologischen Leistungsprofilen bei ADHS und Autismus. Zeitschrift für Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie und Psychotherapie, 35, 95 – 106. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Sternberg, R. J. , Lubart, T. I. (1996). Investing in creativity. American Psychologist, 51, 677 – 688. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Szatmari, P. , Tuff, L. , Finlayson, M. A. , Bartolucci, G. (1990). Asperger’s syndrome and autism: Neurocognitive aspects. Journal .of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 29, 130 – 136. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Torrance, P. E. (1974). Torrance Test of Creative Thinking. Bensenville: Scholastic Testing Service. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Turner, M. A. (1999). Generating novel ideas: Fluency performance in high-functioning and learning disabled individuals with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 40(2), 189 – 201. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Weiß, R. H. , Osterland, J. (1997). Grundintelligenztest Skala 1 (5. Aufl.). Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Weiß, R. H. , Weiß, B. (2006). Grundintelligenztest Skala 2 –Revidierte Fassung (CFT 20-R). Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar