Skip to main content
Original Article

Combining Cognitive and Personality Measures of Impulse Control in the Assessment of Childhood ADHD

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000128

Impulse control is measured both with personality questionnaires and in cognitive laboratory tasks, yet previous results concerning the convergence of these two types of instruments are inconsistent. The current study measured impulse control of children with and without ADHD with a personality questionnaire and a go/no-go task as well as with parent ratings of ADHD symptomatology. Scores on the two measures correlate weakly with one another, yet both correlate moderately to strongly with parent ratings – and both explain unique variance of parent ratings. Accordingly, the simple sum of the standardized scores from the two measures outperforms the single measures in diagnostic accuracy and association with ADHD symptomatology. Results show that a conjoint application of personality and cognitive measures of impulse control is useful for an extended assessment of ADHD. The adequacy of personality and cognitive measures for assessing distinct facets of impulse control is discussed with regard to theoretical models of impulsivity and with regard to ADHD symptomatology.

References

  • American Psychiatric Association . (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV-TR (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Arbeitsgruppe Deutsche Child Behavior Checklist . (1998). Elternfragebogen über das Verhalten von Kindern und Jugendlichen. Deutsche Bearbeitung der Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL/6–18) [German adaptation of the CBCL/6–18]. Köln: Arbeitsgruppe Kinder-, Jugend- und Familiendiagnostik (KJFD). First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Avila, C. , Cuenca, I. , Félix, V. , Parcet, M.-A. , Miranda, A. (2004). Measuring impulsivity in school-aged boys and examining its relationship with ADHD and ODD ratings. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 32, 295–304. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121(1), 65–94. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Berlin, L. , Bohlin, G. , Nyberg, L. , Janols, L.-O. (2004). How well do measures of inhibition and other executive functions discriminate between children with ADHD and controls? Child Neuropsychology, 10, 1–13. doi: 10.1076/chin.10.1.1.26243 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bezdjian, S. , Baker, L. A. , Lozano, D. I. , Raine, A. (2009). Assessing inattention and impulsivity in children during the Go/NoGo task. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 27, 365–383. doi: 10.1348/026151008X314919 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Braet, C. , Claus, L. , Verbeken, S. , Van Vlierberghe, L. (2007). Impulsivity in overweight children. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 16, 473–483. doi: 10.1007/s00787–007–0623–2 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Castellanos, F. X. , Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. , Milham, M. P. , Tannock, R. (2006). Characterizing cognition in ADHD: Beyond executive dysfunction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 117–123. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.01.011 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Döpfner, M. , Lehmkuhl, G. , Steinhausen, H.-C. (2006). KIDS-1: Aufmerksamkeitsdefizit- und Hyperaktivitätsstörung (ADHS) [Diagnostic System for Children – 1 Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)]. Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Doyle, A. E. , Biederman, J. , Seidman, L. J. , Weber, W. , Faraone, S. V. (2000). Diagnostic efficiency of neuropsychological test scores for discriminating boys with and without attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 477–488. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.68.3.477 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Drechsler, R. , Rizzo, P. , Steinhausen, H.-C. (2009). Zur klinischen Validität einer computergestützten Aufmerksamkeitstestbatterie für Kinder (KITAP) bei 7- bis 10-jährigen Kindern mit ADHS [The clinical validity of a computerized test battery for attentional performance for children (KITAP) in the diagnosis of ADHD in children aged 7–10 years]. Kindheit und Entwicklung, 18, 153–161. doi: 10.1026/0942-5403.18.3.153 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Duckworth, A. L. , Kern, M. L. (2011). A meta-analysis of the convergent validity of self-control measures. Journal of Research in Personality, 45, 259–268. doi: 10.1016/ j.jrp.2011.02.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Edmonds, G. W. , Bogg, T. , Roberts, B. W. (2009). Are personality and behavioral measures of impulse control convergent or distinct predictors of health behaviors? Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 806–814. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2009.06.006 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Enticott, P. , Ogloff, J. , Bradshaw, J. (2006). Associations between laboratory measures of executive inhibitory control and self-reported impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 41, 285–294. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2006.01.011 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Evenden, J. L. (1999). Varieties of impulsivity. Psychopharmacology, 146, 348–361. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Eysenck, S. B. G. , Easting, G. , Pearson, P. R. (1984). Age norms for impulsiveness, venturesomeness and empathy in children. Personality and Individual Differences, 5, 315–321. doi: 10.1016/0191–8869(84)90070–9 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Grodzinsky, G. M. , Barkley, R. A. (1999). Predictive power of frontal lobe tests in the diagnosis of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 13, 12–21. doi: 10.1076/clin.13.1.12.1983 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hoza, B. , Waschbusch, D. A. , Owens, J. S. , Pelham, W. E. , Kipp, H. (2001). Academic task persistence of normally achieving ADHD and control boys: Self-evaluations, and attributions. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 69, 271–283. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.69.2.271 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lambek, R. , Tannock, R. , Dalsgaard, S. , Trillingsgaard, A. , Damm, D. , Thomsen, P. H. (2010). Validating neuropsychological subtypes of ADHD: How do children with and without an executive function deficit differ? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 895–904. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02248.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Liddle, E. B. , Hollis, C. , Batty, M. J. , Groom, M. J. , Totman, J. J. , Liotti, M. , ... Liddle, P. F. (2011). Task-related default mode network modulation and inhibitory control in ADHD: Effects of motivation and methylphenidate. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 761–771. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02333.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McNeil, B. J. , Hanley, J. A. (1982). The meaning and use of the area under a receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve. Radiology, 143, 29–36. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Moffitt, T. E. , Arseneault, L. , Belsky, D. , Dickson, N. , Hancox, R. J. , Harrington, H. , ... Caspi, A. (2011). A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 2693–2698. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010076108 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Nigg, J. T. , Willcutt, E. G. , Doyle, A. E. , Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2005). Causal heterogeneity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Do we need neuropsychologically impaired subtypes? Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1224–1230. doi: 10.1016/ j.biopsych.2004.08.025 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Owens, J. S. , Goldfine, M. E. , Evangelista, N. M. , Hoza, B. , Kaiser, N. M. (2007). A critical review of self-perceptions and the positive illusory bias in children with ADHD. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 10, 335–351. doi: 10.1007/s10567–007–0027–3 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Petermann, F. , Petermann, U. (2007). Hamburg-Wechsler-Intelligenztest für Kinder (HAWIK-IV) [Hamburg-Wechsler Intelligence Test for Children (HAWIK-IV)]. Bern: Huber. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Reynolds, B. , Ortengren, A. , Richards, J. B. , de Wit, H. (2006). Dimensions of impulsive behavior: Personality and behavioral measures. Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 305–315. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.03.024 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Robin, X. , Turck, N. , Hainard, A. , Tiberti, N. , Lisacek, F. , Sanchez, J.-C. , Müller, M. (2011). pROC: An open-source package for R and S+ to analyze and compare ROC curves. BMC Bioinformatics, 12, 77. doi: 10.1186/1471-2105-12-77 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Romer, D. , Betancourt, L. , Giannetta, J. M. , Brodsky, N. L. , Farah, M. , Hurt, H. (2009). Executive cognitive functions and impulsivity as correlates of risk taking and problem behavior in preadolescents. Neuropsychologia, 47, 2916–2926. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.06.019 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Scime, M. , Norvilitis, J. M. (2006). Task performance and response to frustration in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychology in the Schools, 43, 377–386. doi: 10.1002/pits.20151 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2005). Causal models of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: From common simple deficits to multiple developmental pathways. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1231–1238. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.09.008 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stadler, C. , Schmeck, K. , Janke, W. (2004). Inventar zur Erfassung von Impulsivität, Risikoverhalten und Empathie bei 9-bis 14-jährigen Kindern: IVE [Inventory for the assessment of impulsivity, risky behavior, and empathy in children aged 9 to 14 years: IVE]. Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Whiteside, S. , Lynam, D. (2001). The Five Factor Model and impulsivity: Using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 669–689. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00064–7 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Willcutt, E. G. , Doyle, A. E. , Nigg, J. T. , Faraone, S. V. , Pennington, B. F. (2005). Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1336–1346. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.02.006 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Winstanley, C. A. , Eagle, D. M. , Robbins, T. W. (2006). Behavioral models of impulsivity in relation to ADHD: Translation between clinical and preclinical studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 26, 379–395. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.01.001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Woerner, W. , Becker, A. , Rothenberger, (2004). Normative data and scale properties of the German parent SDQ. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 13, 11–16. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar